Multiple roles of motor imagery during action observation

Over the last 20 years, the topics of action observation (AO) and motor imagery (MI) have been largely studied in isolation from each other, despite the early integrative account by Jeannerod (1994, 2001). Recent neuroimaging studies demonstrate enhanced cortical activity when AO and MI are performed concurrently (“AO+MI”), compared to either AO or MI performed in isolation. These results indicate the potentially beneficial effects of AO+MI, and they also demonstrate that the underlying neurocognitive processes are partly shared. We separately review the evidence for MI and AO as forms of motor simulation, and present two quantitative literature analyses that indeed indicate rather little overlap between the two bodies of research. We then propose a spectrum of concurrent AO+MI states, from congruent AO+MI where the contents of AO and MI widely overlap, over coordinative AO+MI, where observed and imagined action are different but can be coordinated with each other, to cases of conflicting AO+MI. We believe that an integrative account of AO and MI is theoretically attractive, that it should generate novel experimental approaches, and that it can also stimulate a wide range of applications in sport, occupational therapy, and neurorehabilitation.

[1]  A. Guillot,et al.  Imagining is Not Doing but Involves Specific Motor Commands: A Review of Experimental Data Related to Motor Inhibition , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[2]  D. Sines Towards integration. , 1990, Nursing.

[3]  M. Jeannerod Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.

[4]  G. Buccino,et al.  Action observation versus motor imagery in learning a complex motor task: A short review of literature and a kinematics study , 2013, Neuroscience Letters.

[5]  Frank Van Overwalle,et al.  Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis , 2009, NeuroImage.

[6]  P. Holmes,et al.  Mental practice: neuroscientific support for a new approach , 2011 .

[7]  Claire Calmels,et al.  A Neuroscientific Review of Imagery and Observation Use in Sport , 2008, Journal of motor behavior.

[8]  S. Vogt,et al.  From visuo-motor interactions to imitation learning: Behavioural and brain imaging studies , 2007, Journal of sports sciences.

[9]  R. Schubotz Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[10]  A. Ziegler,et al.  Observation and execution of upper-limb movements as a tool for rehabilitation of motor deficits in paretic stroke patients: protocol of a randomized clinical trial , 2012, BMC Neurology.

[11]  A. Guillot,et al.  Re-imagining motor imagery: building bridges between cognitive neuroscience and sport psychology. , 2012, British journal of psychology.

[12]  V. Pomeroy,et al.  Observation-to-Imitate Plus Practice Could Add Little to Physical Therapy Benefits Within 31 Days of Stroke , 2013, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[13]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  Imitation and observational learning of hand actions: Prefrontal involvement and connectivity , 2012, NeuroImage.

[14]  C. Richards,et al.  Brain activity during visual versus kinesthetic imagery: An fMRI study , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[15]  Aymeric Guillot,et al.  Understanding the timing of motor imagery: recent findings and future directions , 2012 .

[16]  X. Sanchez,et al.  Mental Rehearsal in Table Tennis Performance , 1994, Perceptual and motor skills.

[17]  Simone Rossi,et al.  Cortico-Cortical Connectivity between Right Parietal and Bilateral Primary Motor Cortices during Imagined and Observed Actions: A Combined TMS/tDCS Study , 2011, Front. Neural Circuits.

[18]  M. Erb,et al.  Activation of Cortical and Cerebellar Motor Areas during Executed and Imagined Hand Movements: An fMRI Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Anne Springer,et al.  Action simulation: time course and representational mechanisms , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[20]  J. Decety,et al.  Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta‐analysis , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[21]  W. Prinz,et al.  The imitative mind : development, evolution, and brain bases , 2002 .

[22]  S. Small,et al.  Action observation has a positive impact on rehabilitation of motor deficits after stroke , 2007, NeuroImage.

[23]  H. C. Dijkerman,et al.  Does motor imagery training improve hand function in chronic stroke patients? A pilot study , 2004, Clinical rehabilitation.

[24]  D. Edge Quantitative Measures of Communication in Science: A Critical Review , 1979, History of science; an annual review of literature, research and teaching.

[25]  C. Kilts,et al.  Human functional neuroimaging. , 2012, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[26]  Derek Hoiem,et al.  Action Recognition , 2014, Computer Vision, A Reference Guide.

[27]  Marie Johnston,et al.  Mental practice with motor imagery in stroke recovery: randomized controlled trial of efficacy , 2011, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  C. Winstein,et al.  The Mirror Neuron System: A Neural Substrate for Methods in Stroke Rehabilitation , 2010, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.

[29]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Vegetative response during imagined movement is proportional to mental effort , 1991, Behavioural Brain Research.

[30]  Michael Spittle,et al.  Imagery in Sport , 2005, Sport & Exercise Psychology Review.

[31]  Raymond H. Cuijpers,et al.  Joint Action: Neurocognitive Mechanisms Supporting Human Interaction , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[32]  C. Schuster,et al.  Best practice for motor imagery: a systematic literature review on motor imagery training elements in five different disciplines , 2011, BMC medicine.

[33]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  fMRI Evidence of ‘Mirror’ Responses to Geometric Shapes , 2012, PloS one.

[34]  Ricarda I. Schubotz,et al.  Prediction, Cognition and the Brain , 2009, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[35]  C. Heyes Automatic imitation. , 2011, Psychological bulletin.

[36]  Michael J. Spivey,et al.  Compatibility of motion facilitates visuomotor synchronization. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  Sophie K. Scott,et al.  A little more conversation, a little less action — candidate roles for the motor cortex in speech perception , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[38]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Learning by doing versus learning by thinking: An fMRI study of motor and mental training , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  Maarten J. IJzerman,et al.  Differential cortical activation during observation and observation-and-imagination , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  Simon B Eickhoff,et al.  Brain regions involved in human movement perception: A quantitative voxel‐based meta‐analysis , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[41]  S. Rossi,et al.  Bi-hemispheric effects on corticospinal excitability induced by repeated sessions of imagery versus observation of actions. , 2012, Restorative neurology and neuroscience.

[42]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The timing of mentally represented actions , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[43]  Francine Malouin,et al.  Towards the integration of mental practice in rehabilitation programs. A critical review , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[44]  Davis M. Glasser,et al.  Effects of Action Observation on Physical Training After Stroke , 2008, Stroke.

[45]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[46]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  “Neural efficiency” of experts’ brain during judgment of actions: A high-resolution EEG study in elite and amateur karate athletes , 2010, Behavioural Brain Research.

[47]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Multimodal images in the brain , 2009 .

[48]  D. Wade,et al.  The effects of mental practice in neurological rehabilitation; a systematic review and meta-analysis , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[49]  J. Causer,et al.  Eye gaze metrics reflect a shared motor representation for action observation and movement imagery , 2012, Brain and Cognition.

[50]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[51]  Hae Kyung Park,et al.  Facilitation of Corticospinal Excitability According to Motor Imagery and Mirror Therapy in Healthy Subjects and Stroke Patients , 2011, Annals of rehabilitation medicine.

[52]  L. Stone,et al.  Rehabilitation of hemiparesis after stroke with a mirror , 1999, The Lancet.

[53]  F. Tremblay,et al.  Differential modulation of corticospinal excitability during observation, mental imagery and imitation of hand actions , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[54]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  C. Collet,et al.  Imagery Quality Estimated by Autonomic Response Is Correlated to Sporting Performance Enhancement , 1999, Physiology & Behavior.

[56]  S. Zeki The Mind’s Eye , 2011, Cerebrum : the Dana forum on brain science.

[57]  Aidan Moran,et al.  Measuring Motor Imagery Using Psychometric, Behavioral, and Psychophysiological Tools , 2011, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[58]  S. Vogt,et al.  Task-specific EMG-characteristics during mental training , 1984, Psychological research.

[59]  James M. Kilner,et al.  More than one pathway to action understanding , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[60]  D. Kiper,et al.  Enhanced Activation of Motor Execution Networks Using Action Observation Combined with Imagination of Lower Limb Movements , 2013, PloS one.

[61]  Christian Keysers,et al.  Somatosensation in social perception , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[62]  P. Holmes,et al.  The relationship between corticospinal excitability during motor imagery and motor imagery ability , 2012, Behavioural Brain Research.

[63]  R. Lansing,et al.  Imagination of dynamic exercise produced ventilatory responses which were more apparent in competitive sportsmen. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.

[64]  C Collet,et al.  Duration of Mentally Simulated Movement: A Review , 2005, Journal of motor behavior.

[65]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Motor imagery and action observation: Modulation of sensorimotor brain rhythms during mental control of a brain–computer interface , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[66]  A. Kelly,et al.  Human functional neuroimaging of brain changes associated with practice. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[67]  Tim Shallice,et al.  The Fractionation of Supervisory Control , 2004 .

[68]  Paul E. Downing,et al.  Visuo-motor imagery of specific manual actions: A multi-variate pattern analysis fMRI study , 2012, NeuroImage.

[69]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Mirror neurons: from discovery to autism , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[70]  D. Landers,et al.  The effects of mental practice on motor skill learning and performance: A meta-analysis. , 1983 .

[71]  R. Shepard Ecological constraints on internal representation: resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. , 1984, Psychological review.

[72]  J. Liepert,et al.  Motor excitability during imagination and observation of foot dorsiflexions , 2009, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[73]  Harriet Zuckerman,et al.  Citation analysis and the complex problem of intellectual influence , 1987, Scientometrics.

[74]  S. Vogt Imagery needs preparation too , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[75]  P. Jackson,et al.  The neural network of motor imagery: An ALE meta-analysis , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[76]  Carolyn Copper,et al.  Does mental practice enhance performance , 1994 .

[77]  F. Galton Inquiries into Human Faculty and Its Development , 1883 .

[78]  F. Tremblay,et al.  Corticomotor facilitation associated with observation, imagery and imitation of hand actions: a comparative study in young and old adults , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[79]  Luigi Trojano,et al.  Action observation improves motor imagery: specific interactions between simulative processes , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[80]  Dieter Vaitl,et al.  How equivalent are the action execution, imagery, and observation of intransitive movements? Revisiting the concept of somatotopy during action simulation , 2013, Brain and Cognition.

[81]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[82]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  From mirror neurons to imitation: Facts and speculations , 2002 .

[83]  Karin E. Hinshaw The Effects of Mental Practice on Motor Skill Performance: Critical Evaluation and Meta-Analysis , 1991 .

[84]  John Schwoebel,et al.  The man who executed “imagined” movements: Evidence for dissociable components of the body schema , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[85]  G. Buccino,et al.  Mirror neurons: action observation treatment as a tool in stroke rehabilitation. , 2010, European journal of physical and rehabilitation medicine.

[86]  S. Vogt,et al.  On relations between perceiving, imagining and performing in the learning of cyclical movement sequences. , 1995, British journal of psychology.

[87]  K. Zentgraf,et al.  Cognitive motor processes: The role of motor imagery in the study of motor representations , 2009, Brain Research Reviews.

[88]  D. Freedberg,et al.  Corticomotor Excitability during Observation and Imagination of a Work of Art , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[89]  Stefan Vogt The Imitative Mind: Visuomotor couplings in object-oriented and imitative actions , 2002 .

[90]  M. Schoenfeld,et al.  Action Imagery Combined With Action Observation Activates More Corticomotor Regions Than Action Observation Alone , 2012, Journal of neurologic physical therapy : JNPT.

[91]  R. Weinberg,et al.  Does Imagery Work? Effects on Performance and Mental Skills , 2008 .

[92]  敦命 鈴木 ミラーニューロン(Mirror neuron) , 2010 .

[93]  Tetsuro Muraoka,et al.  Combining observation and imagery of an action enhances human corticospinal excitability , 2009, Neuroscience Research.

[94]  F. Tremblay,et al.  Corticomotor facilitation associated with observation and imagery of hand actions is impaired in Parkinson’s disease , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[95]  Kristen L. Macuga,et al.  Neural representations involved in observed, imagined, and imitated actions are dissociable and hierarchically organized , 2012, NeuroImage.

[96]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[97]  P. Holmes,et al.  An observation-based intervention for stroke rehabilitation: experiences of eight individuals affected by stroke , 2010, Disability and rehabilitation.

[98]  F. Baldissera,et al.  Cyclic time course of motor excitability modulation during the observation of a cyclic hand movement , 2005, Brain Research.

[99]  G Aschersleben,et al.  Correspondence effects with manual gestures and postures: a study of imitation. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[100]  G. Rizzolatti The mirror neuron system and its function in humans , 2005, Anatomy and Embryology.

[101]  K. Funase,et al.  Effect of observation combined with motor imagery of a skilled hand-motor task on motor cortical excitability: Difference between novice and expert , 2012, Neuroscience Letters.

[102]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[103]  M. Goodale,et al.  Two visual systems re-viewed , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[104]  Karen Zentgraf,et al.  Simulation during observation of human actions – Theories, empirical studies, applications , 2011, Vision Research.

[105]  S. Vogt,et al.  Visuomotor priming by pictures of hand postures: perspective matters , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[106]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The role of motor simulation in action perception: a neuropsychological case study , 2009, Psychological research.

[107]  C. Frith The psychology of volition , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[108]  Dieter Vaitl,et al.  Neural simulation of actions: Effector‐ versus action‐specific motor maps within the human premotor and posterior parietal area? , 2014, Human brain mapping.

[109]  R. Shepard Ecological constraints on internal representation: resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. , 1984, Psychological review.

[110]  C. Frith,et al.  Follow you, Follow me: Continuous Mutual Prediction and Adaptation in Joint Tapping , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[111]  Claudio Del Percio,et al.  Judgment of actions in experts: A high-resolution EEG study in elite athletes , 2009, NeuroImage.

[112]  Angela R. Laird,et al.  ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain , 2010, NeuroImage.

[113]  Stefan Geyer,et al.  Imagery of voluntary movement of fingers, toes, and tongue activates corresponding body-part-specific motor representations. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[114]  C. Frith,et al.  The Neural Basis of Mentalizing , 2006, Neuron.

[115]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Involved in the Recognition of Actions Performed by Nonconspecifics: An fMRI Study , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[116]  S. McDonough,et al.  The adjunctive role of mental practice in the rehabilitation of the upper limb after hemiplegic stroke: a pilot studya , 2004, Clinical rehabilitation.

[117]  P. Haggard,et al.  The rubber hand illusion revisited: visuotactile integration and self-attribution. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[118]  C. Frith,et al.  Mechanisms of social cognition. , 2012, Annual review of psychology.

[119]  G. Pezzulo,et al.  Action simulation in the human brain: Twelve questions , 2013 .

[120]  J. Annett On knowing how to do things: a theory of motor imagery. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[121]  Paul Holmes,et al.  Investigating Central Mechanisms Underlying the Effects of Action Observation and Imagery Through Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation , 2011, Journal of motor behavior.

[122]  J B Poline,et al.  Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[123]  C. Heyes,et al.  Science Perspectives on Psychological Mirror Neuron Forum on Behalf Of: Association for Psychological Science , 2022 .

[124]  Nadim Joni Shah,et al.  Prefrontal involvement in imitation learning of hand actions: Effects of practice and expertise , 2007, NeuroImage.

[125]  M. Gazzaniga The cognitive neurosciences, 3rd edition , 2004 .

[126]  Rick Grush,et al.  The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[127]  M. Diamond,et al.  Primary Motor and Sensory Cortex Activation during Motor Performance and Motor Imagery: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[128]  Stefan Vogt,et al.  The Activation of Effect Codes in Response Preparation: New Evidence from an Indirect Priming Paradigm , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[129]  P. Jackson,et al.  Modulation of Brain Activity during Action Observation: Influence of Perspective, Transitivity and Meaningfulness , 2011, PloS one.

[130]  W. Prinz,et al.  Compatibility between Observed and Executed Finger Movements: Comparing Symbolic, Spatial, and Imitative Cues , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[131]  B. Johansson,et al.  Multisensory Stimulation in Stroke Rehabilitation , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[132]  Richard Ramsey,et al.  Incongruent imagery interferes with action initiation , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[133]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[134]  S. Page,et al.  Mental Practice in Chronic Stroke: Results of a Randomized, Placebo-Controlled Trial , 2007, Stroke.

[135]  Stefan Vogt,et al.  Automatic Imitation in Rhythmical Actions: Kinematic Fidelity and the Effects of Compatibility, Delay, and Visual Monitoring , 2012, PloS one.

[136]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[137]  S. Vogt,et al.  Imagery and perception-action mediation in imitative actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[138]  S. W. Woolgar The Identification and Definition of Scientific Collectivities , 1976 .

[139]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system , 2007, Cognitive Processing.

[140]  Karin D. Knorr-Cetina,et al.  Scientific Communities or Transepistemic Arenas of Research? A Critique of Quasi-Economic Models of Science , 1982 .

[141]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[142]  Jonathan D. Nelson,et al.  Human cortical representations for reaching: Mirror neurons for execution, observation, and imagery , 2007, NeuroImage.

[143]  C. Collet,et al.  Autonomic nervous system correlates in movement observation and motor imagery , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[144]  T. Mulder Motor imagery and action observation: cognitive tools for rehabilitation , 2007, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[145]  S. Small,et al.  Fine modulation in network activation during motor execution and motor imagery. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.