The contribution of general and specific motor inhibitory sets to the so-called auditory inhibition of return

The detection of sounds that come from a region of space recently exposed to acoustic stimulation is often slower than the detection of sounds coming from regions of space previously unexposed to acoustic stimulation. The relative increase in reaction time (RT) to targets in recently stimulated locations is generally termed “inhibition of return” (IOR). This term alludes to the possibility that spatial attention is biased against returning to recently visited locations, thus favoring the sampling of new sources of information. However, auditory IOR effects found in paradigms where subjects have to detect a first sound (cue) without making an overt response to it, and then respond as fast as possible to a second sound (target), may be due to a purely motor inhibition carried over from cue to target. Such motor inhibition has been shown to be maximal when cue and target belong to the same category, such as when they occupy the same spatial position. We have assessed the possible contribution of this motor inhibition to auditory IOR effects by having subjects respond to both cues and targets randomly presented in a right location and a left location. Reaction time to targets preceded by cues at the same location was longer than reaction times to targets preceded by cues at the opposite location (IOR effect). Compared to a condition in which subjects responded only to targets, the IOR effect was smaller, but still significant, in the double response condition, suggesting that such an effect depends on both motor inhibition and other factors, possibly related to covert spatial orienting and oculomotor control. A second experiment indicated that the IOR effect component independent of motor inhibition was slightly but significantly greater when space was relevant to the task because subjects had to report the positions of both cues and targets, compared to when space was irrelevant to the task because subjects were not required to report stimulus positions.

[1]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  Oculomotor activity and visual spatial attention , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[2]  G. Tassinari,et al.  Covert orienting to non-informative cues: reaction time studies , 1995, Behavioural Brain Research.

[3]  G. Tassinari,et al.  Ipsilateral inhibition and contralateral facilitation of simple reaction time to non-foveal visual targets from non-informative visual cues. , 1989, Acta psychologica.

[4]  Raymond M. Klein,et al.  On the causes and effects of inhibition of return , 1998 .

[5]  R. Rafal,et al.  The neurology of inhibition: Integrating controlled and automatic processes. , 1994 .

[6]  Jon Driver,et al.  Inhibition of return following an auditory cue The role of central reorienting events , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Volitional Covert Orienting to a Peripheral Cue Does Not Suppress Cue-induced Inhibition of Return , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[8]  Jay Pratt,et al.  The Time to Detect Targets at Inhibited and Noninhibited Locations : Preliminary Evidence for Attentional Momentum , 2004 .

[9]  Nigel Harvey,et al.  Non-Informative Effects of Stimuli Functioning as Cues , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  P. Quinlan,et al.  Sequential effects in rudimentary auditory and visual tasks , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  S. Pollmann,et al.  Covert Reorienting and Inhibition of Return: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  G. Tassinari,et al.  Consequences of covert orienting to non-informative stimuli of different modalities: A unitary mechanism? , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  D. Gitelman,et al.  Covert Visual Spatial Orienting and Saccades: Overlapping Neural Systems , 2000, NeuroImage.

[14]  P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  What is inhibited in inhibition of return? , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  W. Schmidt,et al.  ‘Inhibition of return’ without visual input , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Elizabeth A. Maylor,et al.  Components of orienting in visual space , 1983 .

[17]  Jon Driver,et al.  Inhibition of return is supramodal: a demonstration between all possible pairings of vision, touch, and audition , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  C. A. Marzi,et al.  Distribution in the visual field of the costs of voluntarily allocated attention and of the inhibitory after-effects of covert orienting , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  R. Klein,et al.  Visual and motor effects in inhibition of return. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  M. Posner,et al.  Inhibition of return : Neural basis and function , 1985 .

[21]  J Driver,et al.  Auditory and audiovisual inhibition of return , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A Common Network of Functional Areas for Attention and Eye Movements , 1998, Neuron.

[23]  Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  Auditory cues and inhibition of return: the importance of oculomotor activation , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  P. Calabresi,et al.  Saccade preparation inhibits reorienting to recently attended locations. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  M. Posner,et al.  Components of visual orienting , 1984 .

[26]  Robert Hockey,et al.  Effects of repetition on the facilitatory and inhibitory components of orienting in visual space , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  R. Klein,et al.  Inhibition of return , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  J. Pratt,et al.  Examining the effect of practice on inhibition of return in static displays , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Inhibition of return and oculomotor control in the blind , 2000, Neuroreport.

[30]  E. Maylor,et al.  Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  L. M. Ward,et al.  Spatial relevance determines facilitatory and inhibitory effects of auditory covert spatial orienting , 1999 .

[32]  M. Cheal,et al.  Inhibition of return: support for generality of the phenomenon. , 1999, The Journal of general psychology.