Recall discriminability: Utility of a new CVLT–II measure in the differential diagnosis of dementia

Memory tests that are in a recall format have almost universally measured accuracy in terms of the number of target items reported by the examinee. However, this traditional scoring method can, in certain cases, result in artificially inflated memory accuracy scores. That is, just as a “yes” response bias and high false-positive rate on recognition testing can artificially inflate a patient's hit rate, so, too, a liberal response bias and high intrusion rate on recall testing can artificially inflate a patient's level of target recall. Recognition tests correct for this problem by using a discriminability measure that provides a single score of hit rate relative to false-positive rate; however, recall tests rarely provide a single score of recall accuracy that corrects for intrusion rate. In the present study, we examined the utility of a new recall discriminability measure that analyzes target recall relative to intrusion rate. Patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) or Huntington's disease (HD) were administered the CVLT–II, which provides both the traditional measure of target recall and a new measure of recall discriminability. The results indicate that the new recall discriminability measure was superior to the traditional level of target recall measure in distinguishing the recall performance of AD and HD patients. Implications of these results for clinical practice and theories of memory disorder in dementia are discussed. (JINS, 2005, 11, 708–715.)

[1]  Y. Agid,et al.  Explicit Memory, Procedural Learning and Lexical Priming in Alzheimer's Disease , 1994, Cortex.

[2]  M D'Esposito,et al.  Neuropsychological and neuroanatomical correlates of confabulation. , 1995, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[3]  M. Folstein,et al.  Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease , 1984, Neurology.

[4]  Pattern of intrusions in verbal recall: Comparison of Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease, and frontal lobe dementia , 2001, Brain and Cognition.

[5]  D. Delis,et al.  Verbal memory errors in Alzheimer's and Huntington's dementias , 1988 .

[6]  O. Selnes A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests , 1991, Neurology.

[7]  June Corwin,et al.  On measuring discrimination and response bias: Unequal numbers of targets and distractors and two classes of distractors. , 1994 .

[8]  N Butters,et al.  Clinical assessment of memory disorders in amnesia and dementia. , 1995, Annual review of psychology.

[9]  M. Albert,et al.  Perseveration in behavioral neurology , 1987, Neurology.

[10]  Recall failure following successful generation and recognition of responses by alcoholic Korsakoff patients , 1982, Brain and Cognition.

[11]  D. Salmon,et al.  Intrusion errors in the figural memory of patients with Alzheimer's and Huntington's disease. , 1990, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[12]  L. Cermak,et al.  Memory disorders associated with huntington's disease: Verbal recall, verbal recognition and procedural memory , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[14]  A. Saykin,et al.  Differential lateralization of memory discrimination and response bias in temporal lobe epilepsy patients , 1998, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[15]  J. DeLuca Predicting Neurobehavioral Patterns Following Anterior Communicating Artery Aneurysm , 1993, Cortex.

[16]  D. Delis,et al.  The subcortical dysfunction hypothesis of memory deficits in depression: neuropsychological validation in a subgroup of patients. , 1992, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[17]  K. Heilman,et al.  Testing memory for self-generated items in dementia , 2000, Neurology.

[18]  G. Dalla Barba Different patterns of confabulation. , 1993, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[19]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[20]  A. Schnider,et al.  The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[21]  R. Katzman.,et al.  Intrusions as a sign of Alzheimer dementia chemical and pathological verification , 1982, Annals of neurology.

[22]  Dean C. Delis,et al.  Verbal learning and memory in language impaired children , 1992, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  Robert A. Bornstein,et al.  The Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised , 1990 .

[24]  D. Delis,et al.  The California verbal learning test , 2016 .

[25]  D. Delis,et al.  Profiles of demented and amnesic patients on the California verbal learning test : implications for the assessment of memory disorders , 1991 .