Choice and response time processes in the identification and categorization of unidimensional stimuli
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] David H. Brainard,et al. What makes a categorization task difficult? , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[2] L M Ward,et al. Stimulus information and sequential dependencies in magnitude estimation and cross-modality matching. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[3] R. Teghtsoonian,et al. On the exponents in Stevens' law and the constant in Ekman's law. , 1971, Psychological review.
[4] James L. McClelland. On the time relations of mental processes: An examination of systems of processes in cascade. , 1979 .
[5] N I Durlach,et al. Intensity perception. I. Preliminary theory of intensity resolution. , 1969, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[6] M. Treisman,et al. American Psychological Association, Inc, A Theory of Criterion Setting With an Application to Sequential Dependencies , 2022 .
[7] Walter Schneider,et al. Micro Experimental Laboratory: An integrated system for IBM PC compatibles , 1988 .
[8] Yves Lacouture,et al. The Roles of Stimulus and Response Set Size in the Identification and Categorisation of Unidimensional Stimuli , 1998 .
[9] A. A. J. Marley,et al. A Connectionist Model of Choice and Reaction Time in Absolute Identification , 1991 .
[10] S. Parker,et al. Top-down gain control in the auditory system: Evidence from identification and discrimination experiments , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[11] R M Nosofsky,et al. Comparing exemplar-retrieval and decision-bound models of speeded perceptual classification , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[12] Richard D. Morey,et al. Learning in a unidimensional absolute identification task , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[13] Ashby,et al. A Stochastic Version of General Recognition Theory. , 2000, Journal of mathematical psychology.
[14] Yves Lacouture,et al. A mapping model of bow effects in absolute identification , 1995 .
[15] Donald Laming,et al. The relativity of ‘absolute’ judgements , 1984 .
[16] J. D. Balakrishnan,et al. Comparing the biased choice model and multidimensional decision bound models of identification , 1992 .
[17] Lawrence M. Ward,et al. Response system processes in absolute judgment , 1971 .
[18] K. Lamberts. Information-accumulation theory of speeded categorization. , 2000, Psychological review.
[19] R. Nosofsky,et al. Predicting similarity and categorization from identification. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[20] G. A. Miller. THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .
[21] N I Durlach,et al. Intensity perception. XIII. Perceptual anchor model of context-coding. , 1984, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[22] D. Laming. The measurement of sensation , 1997 .
[23] G. Mandler,et al. Retrieval processes in recognition , 1974, Memory & cognition.
[24] Y. Lacouture,et al. Stimulus modality and stimulus-response compatibility in absolute identification. , 1997, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[25] Alan L. Yuille,et al. Winner-take-all mechanisms , 1998 .
[26] N. I. Durlach,et al. Intensity Perception. II. Resolution in One‐Interval Paradigms , 1972 .
[27] Lawrence M. Ward,et al. Sequential effects and memory in category judgments. , 1970 .
[28] N. Cohen. The Relativity of Absolute Judgments , 1937 .
[29] G. Lockhead,et al. Sequential effects in absolute judgments of loudness , 1968 .
[30] Y. Lacouture,et al. Bow, range, and sequential effects in absolute identification: A response-time analysis , 1997, Psychological research.
[31] W. Hockley. Analysis of response time distributions in the study of cognitive processes. , 1984 .
[32] James L. McClelland,et al. The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.
[33] D. M. Green,et al. The bow and sequential effects in absolute identification , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.
[34] Robert M. Nosofsky,et al. An extension of the exemplar-based random-walk model to separable-dimension stimuli , 2003 .
[35] N. Durlach,et al. Intensity perception. IV. Resolution in roving-level discrimination. , 1973, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[36] L. Braida,et al. Intensity perception. VII. Further data on roving-level discrimination and the resolution and bias edge effects. , 1977, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[37] N. ROUDER,et al. Learning in a unidimensional absolute identification task JEFFREY , .
[38] D. Mewhort,et al. Analysis of Response Time Distributions: An Example Using the Stroop Task , 1991 .
[39] T. Zandt,et al. How to fit a response time distribution , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[40] R. Ratcliff,et al. Retrieval Processes in Recognition Memory , 1976 .
[41] D. M. Green,et al. Attention bands in absolute identification , 1976 .
[42] David M. Green,et al. Effects of practice and distribution of auditory signals on absolute identification , 1977 .
[43] William E. Hockley,et al. Tests of serial scanning in item recognition. , 1982 .
[44] Yves Lacouture. Expanding MEL response box to accommodate up to 16 external buttons , 1995 .
[46] Eugene Galanter,et al. Handbook of mathematical psychology: I. , 1963 .
[47] R. Nosofsky,et al. Seven plus or minus two: a commentary on capacity limitations. , 1994, Psychological review.
[48] S. Mori. A limited-capacity response process in absolute identification , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.
[49] W. T. Maddox,et al. Relations between prototype, exemplar, and decision bound models of categorization , 1993 .