Generalization of force-field adaptation in proprioceptively-deafferented subjects
暂无分享,去创建一个
Christophe Bourdin | Jean-Louis Vercher | Lionel Bringoux | Fabrice R. Sarlegna | R. Miall | J. Vercher | F. Sarlegna | C. Bourdin | J. Cole | L. Bringoux | R. Chris Miall | Jonathan D. Cole | Hannah Z. Lefumat
[1] M. Kawato,et al. Visual Feedback Is Not Necessary for the Learning of Novel Dynamics , 2007, PloS one.
[2] Ian M Franks,et al. Updating of an internal model without proprioception: a deafferentation study , 2006, Neuroreport.
[3] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[4] Robert L. Sainburg,et al. Convergent models of handedness and brain lateralization , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[5] Rachael Seidler,et al. Degree of handedness affects intermanual transfer of skill learning , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[6] Wilsaan M Joiner,et al. The training schedule affects the stability, not the magnitude, of the interlimb transfer of learned dynamics. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.
[7] B. Day,et al. Vestibular-evoked postural responses in the absence of somatosensory information. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[8] R. Miall,et al. Evidence for stronger visuo-motor than visuo-proprioceptive conflict during mirror drawing performed by a deafferented subject and control subjects , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[9] Jinsung Wang,et al. Interlimb transfer of novel inertial dynamics is asymmetrical. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[10] P. Gribble,et al. Observed effector-independent motor learning by observing. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.
[11] Christophe Bourdin,et al. To transfer or not to transfer? Kinematics and laterality quotient predict interlimb transfer of motor learning. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.
[12] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[13] W. T. Thach,et al. Throwing while looking through prisms , 2005 .
[14] Jean-Louis Vercher,et al. The contribution of proprioceptive feedback to sensorimotor adaptation , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[15] W. T. Thach,et al. Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[16] D. C. Howell,et al. Comparing an Individual's Test Score Against Norms Derived from Small Samples , 1998 .
[17] A Melendez-Calderon,et al. Force Field Adaptation Can Be Learned Using Vision in the Absence of Proprioceptive Error , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering.
[18] Yohsuke R. Miyamoto,et al. Temporal structure of motor variability is dynamically regulated and predicts motor learning ability , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.
[19] J. Goldberg,et al. Responses of peripheral vestibular neurons to angular and linear accelerations in the squirrel monkey. , 1975, Acta oto-laryngologica.
[20] T. Hortobágyi,et al. Direct and crossed effects of somatosensory electrical stimulation on motor learning and neuronal plasticity in humans , 2015, European Journal of Applied Physiology.
[21] T. Stein,et al. Intermanual transfer characteristics of dynamic learning: direction, coordinate frame, and consolidation of interlimb generalization. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.
[22] C Ghez,et al. Proprioceptive control of interjoint coordination. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.
[23] Jonathan Cole,et al. Living without touch and peripheral information about body position and movement: Studies with deafferented subjects. , 1995 .
[24] Christophe Bourdin,et al. Force-field adaptation without proprioception: Can vision be used to model limb dynamics? , 2010, Neuropsychologia.
[25] M M Cohen. Visual Feedback, Distribution of Practice, and Intermanual Transfer of Prism Aftereffects , 1973, Perceptual and motor skills.
[26] Etienne Guillaud,et al. Prediction of the body rotation-induced torques on the arm during reaching movements: Evidence from a proprioceptively deafferented subject , 2011, Neuropsychologia.
[27] J. Lackner,et al. Congenitally blind individuals rapidly adapt to coriolis force perturbations of their reaching movements. , 2000, Journal of Neurophysiology.
[28] Y. Lamarre,et al. Postural adjustments associated with different unloadings of the forearm: effects of proprioceptive and cutaneous afferent deprivation. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.
[29] D. G. Woolley,et al. Asymmetric interlimb transfer of concurrent adaptation to opposing dynamic forces , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[30] J. Cole,et al. Proprioception in motor learning: lessons from a deafferented subject , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.
[31] Zoubin Ghahramani,et al. Modular decomposition in visuomotor learning , 1997, Nature.
[32] A. P. Georgopoulos,et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of motor cortex: hemispheric asymmetry and handedness. , 1993, Science.
[33] J. Lackner,et al. Motor adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of reaching movements: endpoint but not trajectory adaptation transfers to the nonexposed arm. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[34] J. Vercher,et al. Internally driven control of reaching movements: A study on a proprioceptively deafferented subject , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.
[35] Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Interaction of visual and proprioceptive feedback during adaptation of human reaching movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[36] Pratik K. Mutha,et al. The influence of visual target information on the online control of movements , 2015, Vision Research.
[37] Simon B. Eickhoff,et al. Handedness and effective connectivity of the motor system , 2014, NeuroImage.