Generalization of force-field adaptation in proprioceptively-deafferented subjects

Humans have the remarkable ability to adapt their motor behaviour to changes in body properties and/or environmental conditions, based on sensory feedback such as vision and proprioception. The role of proprioception has been highlighted for the adaptation to new upper-limb dynamics, which is known to generalize to the opposite, non-adapted limb in healthy individuals. Such interlimb transfer seems to depend on sensory feedback, and the present study assessed whether the chronic loss of proprioception precludes interlimb transfer of dynamic adaptation by testing two well-characterized proprioceptively-deafferented subjects. These had to reach toward visual targets with vision of the limb. For both deafferented subjects, we observed adaptation of the dominant arm to Coriolis forces and after-effects on non-dominant arm movements in different movement directions, thus indicating interlimb transfer. Overall, our findings show that motor learning can generalize across limbs and movement directions despite the loss of proprioceptive afferents.

[1]  M. Kawato,et al.  Visual Feedback Is Not Necessary for the Learning of Novel Dynamics , 2007, PloS one.

[2]  Ian M Franks,et al.  Updating of an internal model without proprioception: a deafferentation study , 2006, Neuroreport.

[3]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learned dynamics of reaching movements generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Robert L. Sainburg,et al.  Convergent models of handedness and brain lateralization , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[5]  Rachael Seidler,et al.  Degree of handedness affects intermanual transfer of skill learning , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Wilsaan M Joiner,et al.  The training schedule affects the stability, not the magnitude, of the interlimb transfer of learned dynamics. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  B. Day,et al.  Vestibular-evoked postural responses in the absence of somatosensory information. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  R. Miall,et al.  Evidence for stronger visuo-motor than visuo-proprioceptive conflict during mirror drawing performed by a deafferented subject and control subjects , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  Jinsung Wang,et al.  Interlimb transfer of novel inertial dynamics is asymmetrical. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  P. Gribble,et al.  Observed effector-independent motor learning by observing. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Christophe Bourdin,et al.  To transfer or not to transfer? Kinematics and laterality quotient predict interlimb transfer of motor learning. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms , 2005 .

[14]  Jean-Louis Vercher,et al.  The contribution of proprioceptive feedback to sensorimotor adaptation , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  D. C. Howell,et al.  Comparing an Individual's Test Score Against Norms Derived from Small Samples , 1998 .

[17]  A Melendez-Calderon,et al.  Force Field Adaptation Can Be Learned Using Vision in the Absence of Proprioceptive Error , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Neural Systems and Rehabilitation Engineering.

[18]  Yohsuke R. Miyamoto,et al.  Temporal structure of motor variability is dynamically regulated and predicts motor learning ability , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  J. Goldberg,et al.  Responses of peripheral vestibular neurons to angular and linear accelerations in the squirrel monkey. , 1975, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[20]  T. Hortobágyi,et al.  Direct and crossed effects of somatosensory electrical stimulation on motor learning and neuronal plasticity in humans , 2015, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[21]  T. Stein,et al.  Intermanual transfer characteristics of dynamic learning: direction, coordinate frame, and consolidation of interlimb generalization. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  C Ghez,et al.  Proprioceptive control of interjoint coordination. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[23]  Jonathan Cole,et al.  Living without touch and peripheral information about body position and movement: Studies with deafferented subjects. , 1995 .

[24]  Christophe Bourdin,et al.  Force-field adaptation without proprioception: Can vision be used to model limb dynamics? , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  M M Cohen Visual Feedback, Distribution of Practice, and Intermanual Transfer of Prism Aftereffects , 1973, Perceptual and motor skills.

[26]  Etienne Guillaud,et al.  Prediction of the body rotation-induced torques on the arm during reaching movements: Evidence from a proprioceptively deafferented subject , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  J. Lackner,et al.  Congenitally blind individuals rapidly adapt to coriolis force perturbations of their reaching movements. , 2000, Journal of Neurophysiology.

[28]  Y. Lamarre,et al.  Postural adjustments associated with different unloadings of the forearm: effects of proprioceptive and cutaneous afferent deprivation. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[29]  D. G. Woolley,et al.  Asymmetric interlimb transfer of concurrent adaptation to opposing dynamic forces , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  J. Cole,et al.  Proprioception in motor learning: lessons from a deafferented subject , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  Zoubin Ghahramani,et al.  Modular decomposition in visuomotor learning , 1997, Nature.

[32]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of motor cortex: hemispheric asymmetry and handedness. , 1993, Science.

[33]  J. Lackner,et al.  Motor adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of reaching movements: endpoint but not trajectory adaptation transfers to the nonexposed arm. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  J. Vercher,et al.  Internally driven control of reaching movements: A study on a proprioceptively deafferented subject , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.

[35]  Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Interaction of visual and proprioceptive feedback during adaptation of human reaching movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  Pratik K. Mutha,et al.  The influence of visual target information on the online control of movements , 2015, Vision Research.

[37]  Simon B. Eickhoff,et al.  Handedness and effective connectivity of the motor system , 2014, NeuroImage.