Harmonic Analysis of Social Cognition

In this paper, we argue that some fundamental concepts and tools of signal processing may be effectively applied to represent and interpret social cognition processes. From this viewpoint, individuals or, more generally, social stimuli are thought of as a weighted sum of harmonics with different frequencies: Low frequencies represent general categories such as gender, ethnic group, nationality, etc., whereas high frequencies account for personal characteristics. Individuals are then seen by observers as the output of a filter that emphasizes a certain range of high or low frequencies. The selection of the filter depends on the social distance between the observing individual or group and the person being observed as well as on motivation, cognitive resources and cultural background. Enhancing lowor high-frequency harmonics is not on equal footing, the latter requiring supplementary energy. This mirrors a well-known property of signal processing filters. More generally, in the light of this correspondence, we show that several established results of social cognition admit a natural interpretation and integration in the signal processing language. While the potential of this connection between an area of social psychology and one of information engineering appears considerable (compression, information retrieval, filtering, feedback, feedforward, sampling, aliasing, etc.), in this paper we shall limit ourselves to laying down what we consider the pillars of this bridge on which future research may be founded.

[1]  Chengchun Hao Introduction to Harmonic Analysis , 2016 .

[2]  Michael E. W. Varnum,et al.  The Origin of Cultural Differences in Cognition , 2010 .

[3]  S. Fiske,et al.  Category-Based and Individuating Processes as a Function of Information and Motivation: Evidence from Our Laboratory , 1989 .

[4]  T. Sørensen,et al.  Is threat the only modulator of attentional selectivity? Redefining the Easterbrook hypothesis , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[5]  H. Tajfel,et al.  An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. , 1979 .

[6]  C. Sedikides,et al.  Out-group homogeneity effects in natural and minimal groups. , 1992 .

[7]  A. Maass,et al.  Chameleonic social identities: Context induces shifts in homosexuals' self-stereotyping and self-categorization , 2013 .

[8]  Michael Wilson MRC Psycholinguistic Database , 2001 .

[9]  Miles Hewstone,et al.  Subtyping and Subgrouping: Processes for the Prevention and Promotion of Stereotype Change , 2001 .

[10]  J. Karylowski,et al.  Temporal distance and accessibility of overt and covert trait-aspects in judgments of self and others , 2020, Self and Identity.

[11]  B. Simon On the asymmetry in the cognitive construal of ingroup and outgroup: A model of egocentric social categorization , 1993 .

[12]  Sabine G. Scholl,et al.  Beautiful Mess Effect: Self–Other Differences in Evaluation of Showing Vulnerability , 2018, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[13]  G. Bodenhausen,et al.  Social Categorization and the Perception of Social Groups , 2012 .

[14]  Z. Pylyshyn Is vision continuous with cognition? The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. , 1999, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[15]  A. Dijksterhuis,et al.  Motivated Social Cognition: Need for Closure Effects on Memory and Judgment , 1996 .

[16]  David J. Sparkman,et al.  Individual Differences in Miserly Thinking Predict Endorsement of Racial/Ethnic Stereotypes , 2020 .

[17]  R. Nisbett,et al.  Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition. , 2001, Psychological review.

[18]  E. McKone,et al.  Asia has the global advantage: Race and visual attention , 2010, Vision Research.

[19]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Social Cognition, from Brains to Culture , 1984 .

[20]  G. Gigerenzer,et al.  Intuitive and Deliberate Judgments Are Based on Common Principles This Article Has Been Corrected. See Last Page , 2022 .

[21]  D. Hamilton,et al.  Perceiving persons and groups. , 1996, Psychological review.

[22]  H. Tajfel,et al.  Classification and quantitative judgement. , 1963, British journal of psychology.

[23]  Ralph Hertwig,et al.  Time and moral judgment , 2011, Cognition.

[24]  Sanne ten Oever,et al.  Coarse-to-fine information integration in human vision , 2019, NeuroImage.

[25]  Darren Newtson Attribution and the unit of perception of ongoing behavior. , 1973 .

[26]  C. Judd,et al.  Measures and models of perceived group variability , 1990 .

[27]  C. Sedikides,et al.  Individual Self, Relational Self, Collective Self: Hierarchical Ordering of the Tripartite Self , 2011 .

[28]  Adam B. Moore,et al.  Who Shalt Not Kill? Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity, Executive Control, and Moral Judgment , 2008, Psychological science.

[29]  R. Nisbett,et al.  East-West Differences in Cognitive Style and Social Orientation: Are They Real? , 2019 .

[30]  O. Govorun,et al.  Ego—Depletion and Prejudice: Separating Automatic and Controlled Components , 2006 .

[31]  Y. Trope,et al.  The association between psychological distance and construal level: evidence from an implicit association test. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[32]  Steven L. Neuberg,et al.  A Continuum of Impression Formation, from Category-Based to Individuating Processes: Influences of Information and Motivation on Attention and Interpretation , 1990 .

[33]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Spatial Distance and Mental Construal of Social Events , 2006, Psychological science.

[34]  R. Cooke Real and Complex Analysis , 2011 .

[35]  David Glen Mick,et al.  Inside Consumption : Consumer Motives, Goals, and Desires , 2005 .

[36]  Gayle Kerr,et al.  The elaboration likelihood model: review, critique and research agenda , 2014 .

[37]  N. Ambady,et al.  A dynamic interactive theory of person construal. , 2011, Psychological review.

[38]  H. Wiese,et al.  Recognising other-race faces is more effortful: The effect of individuation instructions on encoding-related ERP Dm effects , 2020, Biological Psychology.

[39]  Claudia E. Cohen,et al.  Person categories and social perception: Testing some boundaries of the processing effect of prior knowledge. , 1981 .

[40]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Effects of Arbitrarily Placed Category Boundaries on Similarity Judgments , 1997 .

[41]  Sandra J. Milberg,et al.  Category-based and attribute-based reactions to others: Some informational conditions of stereotyping and individuating processes , 1987 .

[42]  Bryan L. Koenig,et al.  Re-Examining Dominance of Categories in Impression Formation: A Test of Dual-Process Models , 2018, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[43]  J. Turner,et al.  Self-categorization theory , 2012 .

[44]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Construal level theory , 2007 .

[45]  C. Unkelbach,et al.  A general valence asymmetry in similarity: Good is more alike than bad. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[46]  D. Wegner,et al.  Thought suppression. , 2000, Annual review of psychology.

[47]  S. Chaiken The heuristic model of persuasion. , 1987 .

[48]  S. Cappa,et al.  Beyond the abstract—concrete dichotomy: Mode of acquisition, concreteness, imageability, familiarity, age of acquisition, context availability, and abstractness norms for a set of 417 Italian words , 2010, Behavior research methods.

[49]  D. Mackie,et al.  Stereotypes: static abstractions or dynamic knowledge structures? , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[50]  J. Pine,et al.  Chunking mechanisms in human learning , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[51]  Daniel M. Bartels,et al.  Principled moral sentiment and the flexibility of moral judgment and decision making , 2008, Cognition.

[52]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  A Social Identity Approach to Person Memory , 2012, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[53]  K. Vohs,et al.  Case Western Reserve University , 1990 .

[54]  Marc Antonini,et al.  Retina-Inspired Filter , 2018, IEEE Transactions on Image Processing.

[55]  S. Gaertner,et al.  Where the division lies: Common ingroup identity moderates the cross-race facial-recognition effect , 2010 .

[56]  Milan Sonka,et al.  Image Processing, Analysis and Machine Vision , 1993, Springer US.

[57]  Y. Trope,et al.  Interpersonal Similarity as a Social Distance Dimension: Implications for Perception of Others' Actions. , 2008, Journal of experimental social psychology.

[58]  Jens Förster,et al.  How Global Versus Local Perception Fits Regulatory Focus , 2005, Psychological science.

[59]  Michel Tuan Pham,et al.  Promotion and Prevention in Consumer Decision Making: State of the Art and Theoretical Propositions , 2005 .

[60]  Gerd Gigerenzer,et al.  Homo Heuristicus: Why Biased Minds Make Better Inferences , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[61]  G. Pezzulo,et al.  Words as social tools: Language, sociality and inner grounding in abstract concepts. , 2019, Physics of life reviews.

[62]  R. Nisbett,et al.  PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES Attending Holistically Versus Analytically: Comparing the Context Sensitivity of Japanese and Americans , 2004 .

[63]  Y. Trope,et al.  Seeing the forest when entry is unlikely: probability and the mental representation of events. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[64]  Ishani Banerji,et al.  Evidence-based recommendations for designing free-sorting experiments , 2015, Behavior research methods.

[65]  H. Markus,et al.  Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. , 1991 .

[66]  D. Hamilton,et al.  A Categorization Approach to Stereotyping , 2015 .

[67]  Denise Sekaquaptewa,et al.  Solo status, stereotype threat, and performance expectancies: Their effects on women’s performance , 2003 .

[68]  A. Y. Lee,et al.  Stereotype efficiency reconsidered: encoding flexibility under cognitive load? , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[69]  P. Sajda,et al.  Regulation of arousal via online neurofeedback improves human performance in a demanding sensory-motor task , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[70]  Steven L. Neuberg,et al.  Motivational influences on impression formation: outcome dependency, accuracy-driven attention, and individuating processes. , 1987, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[71]  Y. Trope,et al.  Construal-level theory of psychological distance. , 2010, Psychological review.

[72]  Gordon E. Legge,et al.  Human efficiency for recognizing and detecting low-pass filtered objects , 1995, Vision Research.

[73]  S. Madon,et al.  Self-fulfilling Prophecies: Mechanisms, Power, and Links to Social Problems , 2011 .

[74]  M. Snyder On the Self-Fulfilling Nature of Social Stereotypes. , 1977 .

[75]  Robert S. Wyer,et al.  The cognitive representation of persons and groups and its effect on recall and recognition memory , 1984 .

[76]  Abstract and concrete self-evaluative goals. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[77]  Thomas Serre,et al.  A feedforward architecture accounts for rapid categorization , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[78]  Steven L. Neuberg,et al.  Toward capturing the functional and nuanced nature of social stereotypes: An affordance management approach , 2020 .

[79]  T. Farroni,et al.  Race and Color: Two Sides of One Story? Development of Biases in Categorical Perception. , 2017, Child development.

[80]  J. Brigham,et al.  Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review , 2001 .

[81]  J. Uleman,et al.  Spontaneous inferences, implicit impressions, and implicit theories. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[82]  P. Todd,et al.  Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart , 1999 .

[83]  Maja Pantic,et al.  Social signal processing: Survey of an emerging domain , 2009, Image Vis. Comput..

[84]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Perception of Out-Group Homogeneity and Levels of Social Categorization: Memory for the Subordinate Attributes of In-Group and Out-Group Members , 1982 .

[85]  M. Snyder Public Appearances, Private Realities: The Psychology of Self-Monitoring , 1986 .

[86]  Kurt Hugenberg,et al.  The categorization-individuation model: an integrative account of the other-race recognition deficit. , 2010, Psychological review.

[87]  K. Ochsner,et al.  Reintegrating the Study of Accuracy Into Social Cognition Research , 2011 .

[88]  A. Kruglanski,et al.  Persuasion by a Single Route: A View From the Unimodel , 1999 .

[89]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  What do people think they're doing? Action identification and human behavior. , 1987 .

[90]  P. V. Lange,et al.  Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology , 2011 .

[91]  J. Maxwell I. On governors , 1868, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London.

[92]  Marlone D. Henderson When seeing the forest reduces the need for trees: the role of construal level in attraction to choice , 2013 .

[93]  D. Oyserman Culture as situated cognition: Cultural mindsets, cultural fluency, and meaning making , 2011 .

[94]  Y. Trope,et al.  The effect of temporal distance on level of mental construal. , 2002 .

[95]  P. Sackett,et al.  A meta-analysis of gender stereotypes and bias in experimental simulations of employment decision making. , 2015, The Journal of applied psychology.

[96]  C. Unkelbach,et al.  Why Good Is More Alike Than Bad: Processing Implications , 2017, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[97]  Nira Liberman,et al.  The effect of global versus local processing styles on assimilation versus contrast in social judgment. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[98]  Joseph G. Weber,et al.  Person memory in intergroup contexts: Categorization versus individuation. , 1995 .

[99]  L. R. Goldberg Unconfounding situational attributions from uncertain, neutral, and ambiguous ones: A psychometric analysis of descriptions of oneself and various types of others. , 1981 .

[100]  Gün R. Semin,et al.  The linguistic category model, its bases, applications and range , 1991 .

[101]  A. Oliva,et al.  Dr. Angry and Mr. Smile: when categorization flexibly modifies the perception of faces in rapid visual presentations , 1999, Cognition.

[102]  Bertram Gawronski,et al.  Gender Differences in Responses to Moral Dilemmas , 2015, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[103]  M. Brewer,et al.  The primacy of age and sex in the structure of person categories , 1989 .

[104]  C. Judd,et al.  Definition and assessment of accuracy in social stereotypes. , 1993, Psychological review.

[105]  Joseph M. Paxton,et al.  Reflection and Reasoning in Moral Judgment , 2022 .

[106]  J. Dovidio,et al.  Relationship of solo status, academic rank, and perceived distinctiveness to job satisfaction of racial/ethnic minorities. , 1998, The Journal of applied psychology.

[107]  Alison Ledgerwood,et al.  The effects of psychological distance on abstraction: Two meta-analyses. , 2015, Psychological bulletin.

[108]  J. Davidoff,et al.  Exposure to an urban environment alters the local bias of a remote culture , 2012, Cognition.

[109]  Hans Knutsson,et al.  Signal processing for computer vision , 1994 .

[110]  C. Macrae,et al.  Social cognition: thinking categorically about others. , 2000, Annual review of psychology.

[111]  Robin R. Vallacher,et al.  Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. , 1989 .

[112]  L. Jussim,et al.  Stereotype (In)Accuracy in Perceptions of Groups and Individuals , 2015 .

[113]  Klaus Fiedler,et al.  On the relations between distinct aspects of psychological distance: An ecological basis of construal-level theory ☆ , 2012 .

[114]  T. Başar,et al.  A New Approach to Linear Filtering and Prediction Problems , 2001 .

[115]  M. Tomasello Origins of human communication , 2008 .

[116]  B. Payne,et al.  A group is more than the average of its parts: Why existing stereotypes are applied more to the same individuals when viewed in groups than when viewed alone , 2019 .

[117]  G Gigerenzer,et al.  Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality. , 1996, Psychological review.

[118]  Z. Kunda,et al.  Stereotypes and the Construal of Individuating Information , 1993 .

[119]  John T. Cacioppo,et al.  The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion , 1986, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology.

[120]  Michael Gilead,et al.  Above and beyond the concrete: The diverse representational substrates of the predictive brain , 2019, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[121]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[122]  S. Fulero,et al.  Recall for confirming events: Memory processes and the maintenance of social stereotypes ☆ , 1979 .

[123]  M. Rothbart,et al.  Category boundaries and category labels: When does a category name influence the perceived similarity of category members? , 2011 .

[124]  A Tutorial Essay on Spatial Filtering and Spatial Vision , 2009 .

[125]  D. Balota,et al.  Conceptualizing control in social cognition: how executive functioning modulates the expression of automatic stereotyping. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[126]  M. Barreto,et al.  Hidden costs of hiding stigma: Ironic interpersonal consequences of concealing a stigmatized identity in social interactions , 2014 .

[127]  Shelley E. Taylor,et al.  Categorical and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping. , 1978 .

[128]  Luciano Arcuri,et al.  Nomina sunt omina: on the inductive potential of nouns and adjectives in person perception. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[129]  G. Bodenhausen Stereotypes as Judgmental Heuristics: Evidence of Circadian Variations in Discrimination , 1990 .

[130]  John A. Swets,et al.  Signal Detection Theory and ROC Analysis in Psychology and Diagnostics: Collected Papers , 1996 .

[131]  A. Dijker,et al.  Stereotyping and attitudinal effects under time pressure , 1996 .

[132]  Stuart Hannabuss,et al.  The Oxford Compendium of Visual Illusions , 2018 .

[133]  R. Shapley,et al.  The effect of contrast on the transfer properties of cat retinal ganglion cells. , 1978, The Journal of physiology.

[134]  Nira Liberman,et al.  From mind to matter: neural correlates of abstract and concrete mindsets. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[135]  R. Nisbett,et al.  The influence of culture: holistic versus analytic perception , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[136]  E. E. Jones,et al.  The actor and the observer: Divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. , 1972 .

[137]  Randolph Blake,et al.  The role of temporal structure in human vision. , 2005, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.