Effects of Basic Path Properties on Human Path Integration

Abstract We investigated how path integration performance can be influenced by five basic path properties in a Virtual Reality Cube. Participants performed path-completion tasks in hallway paths with up to 12 segments. Distance information was visual, whereas turning angles were specified through vision and body senses. The ridge regression analyses suggested that path integration was affected by the number of segments, overall path length/turning angles, and the correct homing distance. Moreover, an un-correlation paradigm showed that path completion performance might be affected by participants' expectations for the correct homing distance of different paths. Implications on models of path integration were discussed.

[1]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[2]  Hermann J Müller,et al.  Evidence of separable spatial representations in a virtual navigation task. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Visual influence on path integration in darkness indicates a multimodal representation of large-scale space , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  Sarah S. Chance,et al.  Spatial Updating of Self-Position and Orientation During Real, Imagined, and Virtual Locomotion , 1998 .

[5]  R Wehner,et al.  Path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis. , 1988, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  Roberta L. Klatzky,et al.  Human navigation ability: Tests of the encoding-error model of path integration , 1999, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[7]  P Péruch,et al.  Homing in Virtual Environments: Effects of Field of View and Path Layout , 1997, Perception.

[8]  Bruce L. McNaughton,et al.  Human Path Integration by Optic Flow , 2004, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[9]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Searching behaviour of desert ants, genusCataglyphis (Formicidae, Hymenoptera) , 2004, Journal of comparative physiology.

[10]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Visual Homing Is Possible Without Landmarks: A Path Integration Study in Virtual Reality , 2002, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[11]  A. E. Hoerl,et al.  Ridge regression: biased estimation for nonorthogonal problems , 2000 .

[12]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[13]  Ranxiao Frances Wang,et al.  The effect of active selection in human path integration. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[14]  Hanspeter A. Mallot,et al.  Path Complexity Does Not Impair Visual Path Integration , 2006, Spatial Cogn. Comput..

[15]  Reginald G. Golledge,et al.  The Encoding-Error Model of Pathway Completion without Vision , 2010 .

[16]  M Behrmann,et al.  Active control of locomotion facilitates nonvisual navigation. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[17]  Roberta L. Klatzky,et al.  Encoding Spatial Representations Through Nonvisually Guided Locomotion: Tests of Human Path Integration , 1997 .

[18]  F. Petzschner,et al.  Iterative Bayesian Estimation as an Explanation for Range and Regression Effects: A Study on Human Path Integration , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Dissociable cognitive mechanisms underlying human path integration , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  R. Long,et al.  Predicting environmental learning from spatial abilities: An indirect route , 1996 .

[21]  Horst Mittelstaedt,et al.  Homing by Path Integration , 1982 .

[22]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[23]  U. Paul Do Geese Use Path Integration for Walking Home , 1982 .

[24]  William H Warren,et al.  Path Integration from Optic Flow and Body Senses in a Homing Task , 2002, Perception.

[25]  L. Regolin,et al.  Detour behaviour in the domestic chick: searching for a disappearing prey or a disappearing social partner , 1995, Animal Behaviour.

[26]  M. Ernst,et al.  Walking Straight into Circles , 2009, Current Biology.

[27]  Romedi Passini,et al.  The Spatio-Cognitive Abilities of the Visually Impaired Population , 1990 .

[28]  John W. Philbeck,et al.  Remembered landmarks enhance the precision of path integration. , 2005 .

[29]  Wolfgang Alt,et al.  Egocentric path integration models and their application to desert arthropods. , 2005, Journal of theoretical biology.

[30]  Jonathan W. Kelly,et al.  The shape of human navigation: How environmental geometry is used in maintenance of spatial orientation , 2008, Cognition.

[31]  Matthew Collett,et al.  Path integration in insects , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[32]  R. Golledge Wayfinding Behavior: Cognitive Mapping and Other Spatial Processes , 2010 .

[33]  Roy A. Ruddle,et al.  Navigating Large-Scale Desk-Top Virtual Buildings: Effects of Orientation Aids and Familiarity , 1998, Presence.

[34]  A. Etienne Navigation of a Small Mammal by Dead Reckoning and Local Cues , 1992 .