Motion-Based Mechanisms of Illusory Contour Synthesis
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] L. Spillmann,et al. Brightness Matching, Brightness Cancellation, and Increment Threshold in the Ehrenstein Illusion , 1984, Perception.
[2] P. Kellman,et al. Strength of visual interpolation depends on the ratio of physically specified to total edge length , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[3] M Bertamini,et al. Identifying contours from occlusion events , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[4] Michael J. Hawken,et al. Macaque VI neurons can signal ‘illusory’ contours , 1993, Nature.
[5] L. Finkel,et al. Integration of distributed cortical systems by reentry: a computer simulation of interactive functionally segregated visual areas , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[6] Hans Wallach. Über visuell wahrgenommene Bewegungsrichtung , 1935 .
[7] I. Rock,et al. Illusory Contours as the Solution to a Problem , 1979, Perception.
[8] M T Swanston,et al. The Representation of Nonuniform Motion: Induced Movement , 1987, Perception.
[9] P. Kellman,et al. Perception of Partly Occluded Objects and Illusory Figures : Evidence for an Identity Hypothesis , 2004 .
[10] A. Yonas,et al. Kinetic occlusion: Further studies of the boundary-flow cue , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[11] K. Nakayama,et al. Single visual neurons code opposing motion independent of direction. , 1983, Science.
[12] S Petry,et al. Evidence for Independent Processing of Subjective Contour Brightness and Sharpness , 1991, Perception.
[13] K. Nakayama,et al. Relative motion induced between stationary lines , 1978, Vision Research.
[14] Susan T. Dumais,et al. The effects of illumination level and retinal size on the apparent strength of subjective contours , 1976 .
[15] Ennio Mingolla,et al. The role of edges and line-ends in illusory contour formation , 1993, Vision Research.
[16] R. Tennant,et al. Inhibition of leukaemia virus replication by polyadenylic acid. , 1972, Nature: New biology.
[17] G. Orban,et al. Shape and Spatial Distribution of Receptive Fields and Antagonistic Motion Surrounds in the Middle Temporal Area (V5) of the Macaque , 1995, The European journal of neuroscience.
[18] E M Brussell,et al. Sensory information and subjective contour. , 1977, The American journal of psychology.
[19] Rüdiger von der Heydt,et al. A computational model of neural contour processing: Figure-ground segregation and illusory contours , 1993, 1993 (4th) International Conference on Computer Vision.
[20] Robert Sekuler,et al. Simultaneous motion contrast: Velocity, sensitivity and depth response , 1975, Vision Research.
[21] G J Andersen,et al. 2-D contour perception resulting from kinetic occlusion , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.
[22] G. Orban,et al. The spatial distribution of the antagonistic surround of MT/V5 neurons. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.
[23] R. B. Post,et al. A reevaluation of the effect of velocity on induced motion , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.
[24] George A. Kaplan,et al. Kinetic disruption of optical texture: The perception of depth at an edge , 1969 .
[25] R. Born,et al. Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area , 1993, Nature.
[26] M. Wertheimer,et al. A source book of Gestalt psychology. , 1939 .
[27] A H Reinhardt-Rutland,et al. Peripheral Movement, Induced Movement, and Aftereffects from Induced Movement , 1981, Perception.
[28] K. Tanaka,et al. Analysis of local and wide-field movements in the superior temporal visual areas of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[29] H. Jones,et al. Context-dependent interactions and visual processing in V1 , 1996, Journal of Physiology-Paris.
[30] Leif H. Finkel,et al. Object Discrimination Based on Depth-from-Occlusion , 1992, Neural Computation.
[31] R. von der Heydt,et al. Illusory contours and cortical neuron responses. , 1984, Science.
[32] K. Duncker,et al. Über induzierte Bewegung , 1929 .
[33] S. Grossberg,et al. Neural dynamics of form perception: boundary completion, illusory figures, and neon color spreading. , 1985 .
[34] W Gerbino,et al. Illusory Figures Based on Local Kinematics , 1991, Perception.
[35] S. Anstis,et al. Interactions between motion aftereffects and induced movement , 1976, Vision Research.
[36] R. Gregory,et al. Cognitive Contours , 1972, Nature.
[37] Roger B. H. Tootell,et al. Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area , 1992, Nature.
[38] J Allman,et al. Direction- and Velocity-Specific Responses from beyond the Classical Receptive Field in the Middle Temporal Visual Area (MT) , 1985, Perception.
[39] W. B. Thompson,et al. Relative motion: Kinetic information for the order of depth at an edge , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.
[40] A H Reinhardt-Rutland. Aftereffect of Induced Rotation: Separation of Inducing and Static Areas, and Monocular Component , 1983, Perceptual and motor skills.
[41] R. Nijhawan,et al. Visual decomposition of colour through motion extrapolation , 1997, Nature.
[42] P J Kellman,et al. Kinetic subjective contours , 1984, Perception & psychophysics.
[43] Stanley A. Klein,et al. Extrapolation or attention shift? , 1995, Nature.
[44] Romi Nijhawan,et al. Motion extrapolation in catching , 1994, Nature.
[45] W. B. Pillsbury. Beiträge zur Analyse der Gesichtswahrnehmungen , 1900 .
[46] R. Sekuler,et al. Assimilation and contrast in motion perception: Explorations in cooperativity , 1990, Vision Research.
[47] G. Orban,et al. Laminar analysis of motion information processing in macaque V5 , 1989, Brain Research.
[48] Romi Nijhawan,et al. Extrapolation or attention shift? , 1995, Nature.
[49] B L Anderson,et al. Reciprocal interactions between occlusion and motion computations. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[50] G. Orban,et al. Influence of a moving textured background on direction selectivity of cat striate neurons. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.