This study investigated the effectiveness of combining enhanced classroom instruction and intense supplemental intervention for struggling readers in first grade. Further, it compared two supplemental interventions derived from distinct theoretical orientations, examining them in terms of effects on academic outcomes and whether children's characteristics were differentially related to an instructional intervention. One intervention (Proactive Reading) was aligned with behavioral theory and was derived from the model of Direct Instruction. The other intervention (Responsive Reading) was aligned with a cognitive theory and was derived from a cognitive-apprenticeship model. These interventions were provided to small groups of first-grade students at risk for reading difficulties. Students were assessed on various reading and reading-related measures associated with success in beginning reading. Results indicated that (a) first-grade students who were at risk for reading failure and who received supplemental instruction in the Responsive or Proactive interventions scored higher on measures of reading and reading-related skills than students who received only enhanced classroom instruction, (b) enhanced classroom instruction appeared to promote high levels of reading growth for many children at risk for reading failure, (c) the two interventions were essentially equally effective even though they reflected different theoretical perspectives, and (d) children's characteristics did not differentially predict the effectiveness of an intervention.
Este estudio investigo la eficacia de combinar una ensenanza intensiva en el aula y una intervencion suplementaria exhaustiva para los lectores de primer grado con dificultades. Adicionalmente se compararon dos intervenciones suplementarias derivadas de orientaciones teoricas diferentes y se las examino en terminos de sus efectos sobre los resultados academicos y de sus relaciones con las caracteristicas de los ninos. Una intervencion (Lectura Proactiva) pertenecia al marco de la teoria de la conducta y derivaba del modelo de Instruccion Directa. La otra intervencion (Lectura Receptiva) estaba relacionada con una teoria cognitiva y derivaba de un modelo cognitivo de aprendizaje. Las intervenciones se realizaron con pequenos grupos de estudiantes en riesgo de fracaso en lectura que asistian a primer grado. Se evaluo a los estudiantes en varias medidas de lectura y habilidades relacionadas que se asocian al exito en lectura inicial. Los resultados indicaron que: a) los estudiantes de primer grado en riesgo de fracaso en lectura que recibieron intervencion suplementaria, tanto Receptiva como Proactiva, tuvieron mejores calificaciones en medidas de lectura y habilidades relacionadas que los estudiantes que solo recibieron ensenanza intensiva en el aula, b) la ensenanza intensiva en el aula parecio promover altos niveles de desarrollo lector en muchos ninos en riesgo de fracaso, c) los dos tipos de intervencion fueron igualmente eficaces aunque reflejaran perspectivas teoricas diferentes y d) las caracteristicas de los ninos no predijeron en forma diferencial la eficacia de una in-Unterschiedsmerkmae in der Wirksamkeit einer Intervention voraussagen.
Diese Studie untersuchte die Effektivitat im Kombinieren von verstarktem Klassenraumunterricht und intensiver, suppletorischer Intervention bei sich abmuhenden Schulern in der ersten Klasse. Ferner verglich sie zwei suppletorische Interventionen, abgeleitet aus deutlich theoretischen Gesichtspunkten durch Untersuchen von Effekten aufgrund akademischer Auswirkungen und ob die Charakteristiken der Kinder sich abweichend zu einem unterrichteten Anweisungseingriff verhielten. Eine Intervention (pro-aktives Lesen) wurde einer Verhaltenstheorie angeglichen und von dem Modell des Direktunterrichts abgeleitet. Die andere Intervention (responsives Lesen) wurde einer kognitiven Theorie angeglichen und von einem kognitiven Lehrmodell abgeleitet. Diese Interventionen wurden kleinen Gruppen von Schulern mit Risiken zu Leseschwierigkeiten in der ersten Klasse vermittelt. Die Schuler wurden auf verschiedene Lese- und lesebezogenen Masnahmen gepruft, die mit dem Erfolg beim Lesen fur Anfanger verknupft waren. Die Ergebnisse haben gezeigt, dass (a) Schuler der ersten Klasse, bei denen die Gefahr des Leseversagens bestand und die suppletorische Lehranweisungen mittels responsiver oder proaktiver Eingriffe erhielten, hohere Leistungen in der Lesebewertung und bei den lesebezogenen Leistungen erzielten, als jene Schuler die lediglich einen verstarkten Klassenraumunterricht erhielten, (b) verstarkter Unterricht im Klassenraum schien im hohen Grade die Lesesteigerung fur viele Kinder mit Risiken zum Leseversagen zu fordern, (c) die beiden Interventionen waren im wesentlichen gleichermasen wirksam, obwohl sie unterschiedliche theoretische Perspektiven reflektierten, und (d) aus den Charakteristiken der Kinder liesen sich keine Schlusse uber tervencion.
Cette etude a examine l'efficacite de la combinaison d'un enseignement renforce en classe avec une intervention supplementaire intense sur des lecteurs de premiere annee en difficulte. Plus precisement, elle a compare deux interventions supplementaires provenant d'orientations theoriques distinctes, en les examinant en termes d'effets sur les resultats academiques et en fonction des caracteristiques des enfants selon le type d'intervention. Une intervention (Lecture proactive) correspondait a une position behavioriste et etait issue du modele de l'enseignement direct. L'autre intervention (Lecture repondante) correspondait a une theorie cognitiviste et etait issue d'un modele d'apprentissage cognitif. Ces interventions ont ete effectuees aupres de petits groupes d'elives de premiere annee presentant des risques de difficultes en lecture. On a opere differentes mesures de la lecture ou de mesures associees avec la reussite en lecture en debut d'apprentissage. Les resultats ont montre que, a) les eleves de premiere annee qui etaient en risque d'echec en lecture et qui ont recu un enseignement supplementaire sous forme d'intervention en Lecture proactive ou en Lecture repondante ont obtenu de meilleurs resultats que ceux qui ont recu seulement un enseignement renforce en classe, b) un enseignement renforce en classe permet d'atteindre un plus haut niveau de developpement en lecture pour beaucoup d'enfants presentant un risque d'echec en lecture, c) les deux interventions ont ete pour l'essentiel aussi efficaces l'une que l'autre, quoique refletant des perspectives theoriques differentes, et d) les caracteristiques des enfants ne permettent pas de predire de maniere differenciee l'efficacite de telle ou telle intervention.
[1]
Jack M. Fletcher,et al.
Growth in Precursor and Reading–Related Skills: Do Low–Achieving and IQ–Discrepant Readers Develop Differently?
,
2002
.
[2]
Marie M. Clay,et al.
An Observation Survey of Early Literacy Achievement
,
1993
.
[3]
Jack M. Fletcher,et al.
The Role of Instruction in Learning To Read: Preventing Reading Failure in At-Risk Children.
,
1998
.
[4]
Catherine E. Snow,et al.
Preventing reading difficulties in young children
,
1998
.
[5]
Irene C. Fountas.
Matching Books to Readers: Using Leveled Books in Guided Reading, K-3
,
1999
.
[6]
Richard K. Wagner,et al.
Causal relations between the development of phonological processing abilities and the acquisition of reading skills: A meta-analysis.
,
1988
.
[7]
A. Collins,et al.
Situated Cognition and the Culture of Learning
,
1989
.
[8]
Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE IN THE PUBLIC INTEREST HOW PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE INFORMS THE TEACHING OF READING
,
2022
.
[9]
Michelle K. Hosp,et al.
Oral Reading Fluency as an Indicator of Reading Competence: A Theoretical, Empirical, and Historical Analysis
,
2001
.
[10]
L. Vygotsky.
Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes: Harvard University Press
,
1978
.
[11]
Edward R. Sipay,et al.
Cognitive profiles of difficult-to-remediate and readily remediated poor readers : Early intervention as a vehicle for distinguishing between cognitive and experiential deficits as basic causes of specific Reading disability
,
1996
.
[12]
M. Pressley,et al.
Cognition, Teaching, and Assessment
,
1995
.
[13]
Kenneth A. Kavale,et al.
The Character of Learning Disabilities: An Iowa Profile
,
1992
.
[14]
Sharon Vaughn,et al.
How effective are one-to-one tutoring programs in reading for elementary students at risk for reading failure? A meta-analysis of the intervention research
,
2000
.
[15]
Douglas Carnine,et al.
Theory of instruction : principles and applications
,
1982
.
[16]
Rollanda E. O'Connor.
Increasing the Intensity of Intervention in Kindergarten and First Grade
,
2000
.
[17]
J. Downing,et al.
Comparative reading : cross-national studies of behavior and processes in reading and writing
,
1973
.
[18]
L. S. Vygotskiĭ,et al.
Mind in society : the development of higher psychological processes
,
1978
.
[19]
G. Lyon,et al.
Toward a definition of dyslexia
,
1995,
Annals of dyslexia.
[20]
Timothy Shanahan,et al.
On the Effectiveness and Limitations of Tutoring in Reading
,
1998
.
[21]
T. Shanahan.
Chapter 6: On the Effectiveness and Limitations of Tutoring in Reading
,
1998
.
[22]
T. Conway,et al.
Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological processing disabilities: Group and individual responses to instruction.
,
1999
.
[23]
Jack M. Fletcher,et al.
The dimensionality of phonological awareness : An application of item response theory
,
1999
.
[24]
B. Skinner,et al.
Science and human behavior
,
1953
.
[25]
K. Stanovich.
Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy.
,
1986
.
[26]
John W. Schell,et al.
Cognitive Apprenticeship for Learners with Special Needs
,
1994
.
[27]
B. Rogoff.
Apprenticeship in Thinking: Cognitive Development in Social Context
,
1990
.
[28]
Richard L. Allington,et al.
The Reading Instruction Provided Readers of Differing Reading Abilities
,
1983,
The Elementary School Journal.
[29]
G. Whitehurst,et al.
Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: evidence from a longitudinal structural model.
,
2002,
Developmental psychology.
[30]
W. Becker.
Applications of Behavior Principles in Typical Classrooms
,
1973,
Teachers College Record: The Voice of Scholarship in Education.
[31]
D. Langenberg.
Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction
,
2000
.
[32]
Joseph K. Torgesen,et al.
Individual Differences in Response to Early Interventions in Reading: The Lingering Problem of Treatment Resisters
,
2000
.
[33]
M. Pressley,et al.
The Nature of Cognitive Strategy Instruction: Interactive Strategy Construction
,
1991
.
[34]
Jack M. Fletcher,et al.
Developmental Lag versus Deficit Models of Reading Disability: A Longitudinal, Individual Growth Curves Analysis.
,
1996
.
[35]
J. Jaccard,et al.
Towards distinguishing between cognitive and experiential deficits as primary sources of difficulty in learning to read: A two year follow-up of difficult to remediate and readily remediated poor readersConceptualizing behavior in attitude research
,
2002
.
[36]
Joseph K. Torgesen,et al.
Critical Elements of Classroom and Small–Group Instruction Promote Reading Success in All Children
,
2001
.
[37]
Marie M. Clay,et al.
Learning to be learning disabled.
,
1987
.
[38]
C. Juel.
Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades.
,
1988
.
[39]
Patricia G. Mathes,et al.
The prevention and identification of reading disability.
,
2002,
Seminars in pediatric neurology.
[40]
Lynn S. Fuchs,et al.
The Efficacy of Peer Tutoring in Reading for Students with Disabilities: A Best-Evidence Synthesis.
,
1991
.
[41]
Siegfried Engelmann,et al.
Preventing failure in the primary grades
,
1969
.
[42]
Douglas Fuchs,et al.
Peer-Assisted Learning Strategies for First-Grade Readers: Responding to the Needs of Diverse Learners.
,
1998
.
[43]
Marie M. Clay,et al.
Reading Recovery: A Guidebook for Teachers in Training
,
1993
.
[44]
Joseph K. Torgesen,et al.
The Effects of Peer-Assisted Literacy Strategies for First-Grade Readers With and Without Additional Computer-Assisted Instruction in Phonological Awareness
,
2001
.
[45]
M. Stuart.
Learning to read: A longitudinal study
,
1993
.
[46]
J. Torgesen,et al.
All Children Can Learn To Read: Critical Care for the Prevention of Reading Failure
,
1998
.
[47]
Lynn S. Fuchs,et al.
Curriculum-Based Measurement
,
1990
.