Social behavioural adaptation in Autism

Autism is still diagnosed on the basis of subjective assessments of elusive notions such as interpersonal contact and social reciprocity. We propose to decompose reciprocal social interactions in their basic computational constituents. Specifically, we test the assumption that autistic individuals disregard information regarding the stakes of social interactions when adapting to others. We compared 24 adult autistic participants to 24 neurotypical (NT) participants engaging in a repeated dyadic competitive game against artificial agents with calibrated reciprocal adaptation capabilities. Critically, participants were framed to believe either that they were competing against somebody else or that they were playing a gambling game. Only the NT participants did alter their adaptation strategy when they held information regarding others' competitive incentives, in which case they outperformed the AS group. Computational analyses of trial-by-trial choice sequences show that the behavioural repertoire of autistic people exhibits subnormal flexibility and mentalizing sophistication, especially when information regarding opponents’ incentives was available. These two computational phenotypes yield 79% diagnosis classification accuracy and explain 62% of the severity of social symptoms in autistic participants. Such computational decomposition of the autistic social phenotype may prove relevant for drawing novel diagnostic boundaries and guiding individualized clinical interventions in autism.

[1]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[2]  U. Frith The 38th Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture Why we need cognitive explanations of autism , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  M. Nowak,et al.  A strategy of win-stay, lose-shift that outperforms tit-for-tat in the Prisoner's Dilemma game , 1993, Nature.

[4]  M. Dapretto,et al.  Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: the role of prosody and context. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  Morton Ann Gernsbacher,et al.  Toward a Behavior of Reciprocity. , 2006, The journal of developmental processes.

[6]  D. Burr,et al.  When the world becomes ‘too real’: a Bayesian explanation of autistic perception , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  E. Redcay,et al.  Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction , 2019, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[8]  F. Happé The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. , 1995, Child development.

[9]  J. Hohwy,et al.  Bayesian Approaches to Autism: Towards Volatility, Action, and Behavior , 2017, Psychological bulletin.

[10]  C. Frith,et al.  Mechanisms of social cognition. , 2012, Annual review of psychology.

[11]  Ulrich Berger,et al.  Brown's original fictitious play , 2007, J. Econ. Theory.

[12]  F. Happé,et al.  Social anxiety in autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review , 2018, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders.

[13]  Lionel Rigoux,et al.  VBA: A Probabilistic Treatment of Nonlinear Models for Neurobiological and Behavioural Data , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[14]  H. Geurts,et al.  The paradox of cognitive flexibility in autism , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  Catherine Lord,et al.  Standardizing ADOS Domain Scores: Separating Severity of Social Affect and Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors , 2014, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[16]  D. Tekin,et al.  Investigating false belief levels of typically developed children and children with autism , 2010 .

[17]  K. Vogeley,et al.  Toward a second-person neuroscience 1 , 2013, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[18]  Jan Kiesewetter,et al.  Increased Event-Related Potentials and Alpha-, Beta-, and Gamma-Activity Associated with Intentional Actions , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[19]  Philip D. Harvey,et al.  Social Cognition Psychometric Evaluation: Results of the Final Validation Study , 2018, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[20]  Jill Boucher,et al.  Using Tests of False Belief with Children with Autism: How Valid and Reliable are they? , 2001, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.

[21]  Roberta Fadda,et al.  Exploring the Role of Theory of Mind in Moral Judgment: The Case of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[22]  N. Wenderoth,et al.  Beyond Autism: Introducing the Dialectical Misattunement Hypothesis and a Bayesian Account of Intersubjectivity , 2017, Psychopathology.

[23]  Lucia Margari,et al.  A review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder , 2016, Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment.

[24]  Colin F Camerer,et al.  Insensitivity to social reputation in autism , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[25]  F. Happé,et al.  Time to give up on a single explanation for autism , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  Julius Schönherr What’s so Special About Interaction in Social Cognition? , 2017 .

[27]  Marcus P. Adams,et al.  Explaining the theory of mind deficit in autism spectrum disorder , 2013 .

[28]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Reputation Management: Evidence for Ability But Reduced Propensity in Autism , 2013, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[29]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Bayesian model selection for group studies — Revisited , 2014, NeuroImage.

[30]  Bart Verheij,et al.  How much does it help to know what she knows you know? An agent-based simulation study , 2013, Artif. Intell..

[31]  J. Daunizeau,et al.  Theory of Mind: Did Evolution Fool Us? , 2014, PloS one.

[32]  U. Frith,et al.  Autism: beyond “theory of mind” , 1994, Cognition.

[33]  Tony Charman,et al.  Measurement Issues: Screening and diagnostic instruments for autism spectrum disorders - lessons from research and practice. , 2013, Child and adolescent mental health.

[34]  S. Brison The Intentional Stance , 1989 .

[35]  J. Brock,et al.  Alternative Bayesian accounts of autistic perception: comment on Pellicano and Burr , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Observing the Observer (I): Meta-Bayesian Models of Learning and Decision-Making , 2010, PloS one.

[37]  R. Hoffman,et al.  Functional connectivity studies of patients with auditory verbal hallucinations , 2011, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[38]  R. Adolphs The neurobiology of social cognition , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[39]  S. Gallagher Strong interaction and self-agency , 2011 .

[40]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[41]  Leonhard Schilbach,et al.  Towards a second-person neuropsychiatry , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[42]  Koen Ponnet,et al.  Mind-Reading in Young Adults with ASD: Does Structure Matter? , 2008, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[43]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Cooperation and Heterogeneity of the Autistic Mind , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  Sarah J White,et al.  Exploring ‘The autisms’ at a cognitive level , 2016, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[45]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Variational free energy and the Laplace approximation , 2007, NeuroImage.

[46]  L. Schilbach,et al.  Observing and participating in social interactions: Action perception and action control across the autistic spectrum , 2017, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Variational Bayesian identification and prediction of stochastic nonlinear dynamic causal models , 2009, Physica D. Nonlinear phenomena.

[48]  R. Schultz,et al.  The social motivation theory of autism , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[49]  Jean Daunizeau,et al.  The Social Bayesian Brain: Does Mentalizing Make a Difference When We Learn? , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[50]  Francesca Ervas,et al.  Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders Do Not Use Social Stereotypes in Irony Comprehension , 2014, PloS one.

[51]  Jean Daunizeau,et al.  Does the way we read others' mind change over the lifespan? Insights from a massive web poll of cognitive skills from childhood to late adulthood , 2017, Cortex.

[52]  Jean Daunizeau,et al.  Reading wild minds: A computational assay of Theory of Mind sophistication across seven primate species , 2017, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[53]  Christian Keysers,et al.  Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders in Adults: the Use of Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) Module 4 , 2010, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[54]  G. Rees,et al.  Adults with autism over-estimate the volatility of the sensory environment , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  Peter Bossaerts,et al.  Neural correlates of mentalizing-related computations during strategic interactions in humans , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[56]  Albert Newen,et al.  Reconceptualizing second-person interaction , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[57]  Gael Orsmond,et al.  The Symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorders in Adolescence and Adulthood , 2003, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.

[58]  Luca Surian,et al.  Mental State Understanding and Moral Judgment in Children with Autistic Spectrum Disorder , 2016, Frontiers in psychology.

[59]  K. Stephan,et al.  Can Bayesian Theories of Autism Spectrum Disorder Help Improve Clinical Practice? , 2016, Front. Psychiatry.

[60]  Helen McConachie,et al.  Interventions based on the Theory of Mind cognitive model for autism spectrum disorder (ASD). , 2014, The Cochrane database of systematic reviews.

[61]  C. Frith The role of metacognition in human social interactions , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.