Preparation for speeded action as a psychophysiological concept.

Mental preparation aids performance and induces multiple physiological changes that should inform concepts of preparation. To date, however, these changes have been interpreted as being due to a global preparatory process (e.g., attention or alertness). The authors review psychophysiological and performance investigations of preparation. Concepts of the central regulation of action offer an integrative framework for understanding the psychophysiology of preparation. If people process multiple streams of information concurrently, then preparatory processing requires a form of supervisory attention- central regulation to maintain unity of action. This concept is consistent with existing psychophysiological results and links them to current views of information processing. Conversely, psychophysiological measures may provide indices to test concepts within theories of the central regulation of action.

[1]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[2]  M. Mon-Williams,et al.  Motor Control and Learning , 2006 .

[3]  H. Critchley,et al.  Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  E Berendsen,et al.  Goal neglect and inhibitory limitations: dissociable causes of interference effects in conflict situations. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[5]  N. Meiran,et al.  Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  H Pashler,et al.  Shifting visual attention and selecting motor responses: distinct attentional mechanisms. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Jörn Diedrichsen,et al.  Bimanual interference associated with the selection of target locations. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[9]  J. Jennings,et al.  Recall, recognition, and rate: memory and the heart. , 1980, Psychophysiology.

[10]  A. Osman,et al.  Mechanisms of speed–accuracy tradeoff: evidence from covert motor processes , 2000, Biological Psychology.

[11]  W. N. Schoenfeld,et al.  Principles of Psychology , 2007 .

[12]  L. Lange Neue Experimente uber den Vorgang der einfachen Reaction auf Sinneseindrucke , 1888 .

[13]  David A. Rosenbaum,et al.  Reaching and Grasping , 1990 .

[14]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Computations underlying the execution of movement: a biological perspective. , 1991, Science.

[15]  Odmar Neumann Theories of attention. , 1996 .

[16]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[17]  G D Logan,et al.  Constraints on strategy construction in a speeded discrimination task. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  津田 敏雄,et al.  Contingent negative variation , 1984 .

[19]  C. Brunia,et al.  Waiting in readiness: gating in attention and motor preparation. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[20]  E. Soetens,et al.  Expectancy or Automatic Facilitation? Separating Sequential Effects in Two-Choice Reaction Time , 1985 .

[21]  H. Heuer Dual-task performance. , 1996 .

[22]  J. Jolles,et al.  Rapid visuomotor preparation in the human brain: a functional MRI study. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[23]  D. Heslenfeld,et al.  Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation: electrophysiological evidence. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[24]  B. Rockstroh Slow cortical potentials and behavior , 1989 .

[25]  J. Mink THE BASAL GANGLIA: FOCUSED SELECTION AND INHIBITION OF COMPETING MOTOR PROGRAMS , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[26]  D. A. Taylor,et al.  The cuing and priming of cognitive operations. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  Richard Trumbull,et al.  Psychological stress : issues in research , 1967 .

[28]  T. Robbins,et al.  The prefrontal cortex: Executive and cognitive functions. , 1998 .

[29]  Ernst Fernando Lopes Da Silva Niedermeyer,et al.  Electroencephalography, basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields , 1982 .

[30]  R. Näätänen Attention and brain function , 1992 .

[31]  Ritske de Jong,et al.  Strategical determinants of compatibility effects with task uncertainty , 1995 .

[32]  G. Sperling,et al.  Episodic theory of the dynamics of spatial attention. , 1995 .

[33]  Ray Johnson,et al.  Event-related brain potentials : basic issues and applications , 1990 .

[34]  N. Meiran Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.

[35]  Stephen M. Rao,et al.  Motor Sequence Complexity and Performing Hand Produce Differential Patterns of Hemispheric Lateralization , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  Edward Awh,et al.  Top-down control over biased competition during covert spatial orienting. , 2003 .

[37]  Advancing from the ventral striatum to the extended amygdala : implications for neuropsychiatry and drug abuse : in honor of Lennart Heimer , 1999 .

[38]  P E Roland,et al.  Somatotopical tuning of postcentral gyrus during focal attention in man. A regional cerebral blood flow study. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  G. Logan,et al.  Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Hisae Gemba,et al.  Potential related to no-go reaction in go/no-go hand movement with discrimination between tone stimuli of different frequencies in the monkey , 1990, Brain Research.

[41]  Oswald Kuelpe The problem of attention , 1902 .

[42]  D I Boomsma,et al.  Does the heart know what the eye sees? A cardiac/pupillometric analysis of motor preparation and response execution. , 1989, Psychophysiology.

[43]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Attention : selection, awareness, and control : a tribute to Donald Broadbent , 1996 .

[44]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  Can blindness to response-compatible stimuli be observed in the absence of a response? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[46]  M. Mesulam A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect , 1981, Annals of neurology.

[47]  J R Anderson,et al.  Task preparation and task repetition: two-component model of task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[48]  R. Kliegl,et al.  Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[49]  G. Band,et al.  Speed-accuracy modulation in case of conflict: the roles of activation and inhibition , 2003, Psychological research.

[50]  J. Müsseler,et al.  Time course of the blindness to response-compatible stimuli. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Working memory, task switching, and executive control in the task span procedure. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[52]  Rolf Verleger,et al.  Posterior and anterior contribution of hand-movement preparation to late CNV. , 2000 .

[53]  P J Beek,et al.  Dynamical substructure of coordinated rhythmic movements. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[54]  J. Fuster The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .

[55]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Processes of task-set reconfiguration: switching operations and implementation operations. , 2002, Acta psychologica.

[56]  J. Ford,et al.  ERPs to response production and inhibition. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[57]  James J. Gibson,et al.  A critical review of the concept of set in contemporary experimental psychology. , 1941 .

[58]  H. Pashler The Psychology of Attention , 1997 .

[59]  A. Damasio Descartes' error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. avon books , 1994 .

[60]  A. Sanders Towards a model of stress and human performance. , 1983, Acta psychologica.

[61]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Activation of human prefrontal cortex during spatial and nonspatial working memory tasks measured by functional MRI. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[62]  C. V. D. Heuvel,et al.  Intentional and unintentional contributions of nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods. , 2001 .

[63]  R. Forget,et al.  The modulation of somatosensory evoked potentials during the foreperiod of a forewarned reaction time task. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[64]  S. Hackley,et al.  Preparation for Donders' type B and C reaction tasks. , 1990, Acta psychologica.

[65]  K. Willmes,et al.  On the Functional Neuroanatomy of Intrinsic and Phasic Alertness , 2001, NeuroImage.

[66]  A. Thierry,et al.  Motor and Cognitive Functions of the Prefrontal Cortex , 1994, Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences.

[67]  Heikki Lyytinen,et al.  Physiology of preparation: Brain slow waves, heart rate, and respiration preceding triggering in rifle shooting. , 1992 .

[68]  P McLeod,et al.  Does Probe RT Measure Central Processing Demand? , 1978, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[69]  T. Carr,et al.  Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language , 1994 .

[70]  Natalie A. Phillips,et al.  Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions , 2006, Biological Psychology.

[71]  M. Corbetta Functional anatomy of visual attention in the human brain: Studies with positron emission tomography. , 1998 .

[72]  David W. Martin,et al.  Processing demands of encoding: What does secondary task performance reflect? , 1980 .

[73]  A. Nobre,et al.  Orienting Attention to Locations in Internal Representations , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[74]  J. Beatty Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. , 1982 .

[75]  E. Zarahn,et al.  The Role of Prefrontal Cortex in Sensory Memory and Motor Preparation: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2000, NeuroImage.

[76]  M. S. Mayzner,et al.  Cognition And Reality , 1976 .

[77]  James H. Abbs,et al.  Motor Coordination for Functional Human Behaviors: Perspectives From A Speech Motor Data Base , 1989 .

[78]  R. Spinks The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology, and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe, 3rd ed. , 2000 .

[79]  Bruce D. McCandliss,et al.  Testing the Efficiency and Independence of Attentional Networks , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[80]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Brain Mapping: The Methods , 2002 .

[81]  G. Schwartz,et al.  Consciousness and Self-Regulation , 1976 .

[82]  M. Raichle,et al.  Blood flow changes in human somatosensory cortex during anticipated stimulation , 1995, Nature.

[83]  Neurophysiology and Psychophysiology , 2021 .

[84]  S. Los,et al.  Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[85]  C. Brunia Movement and stimulus preceding negativity , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[86]  S. Hackley,et al.  Which stages of processing are speeded by a warning signal? , 2003, Biological Psychology.

[87]  M. Coltheart,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of anticipatory and poststimulus components of task switching. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[88]  E. Donchin,et al.  Detecting early communication: Using measures of movement-related potentials to illuminate human information processing , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[89]  J. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.

[90]  L. Karlin Reaction time as a function of foreperiod duration and variability. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[91]  J. Pratt,et al.  Dissociating visual attention and effector selection in spatial precuing tasks. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[92]  M. W. Molen,et al.  A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm , 2001, Biological Psychology.

[93]  H. Pashler Dual-task interference and elementary mental mechanisms , 1993 .

[94]  Cornelis H. M. Brunia,et al.  Reflexes as a tool: A window in the central nervous system , 1988 .

[95]  S Monsell,et al.  Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task? , 2000, Psychological research.

[96]  Keith J. Holyoak,et al.  Structure and Functions of the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 1996 .

[97]  N. Yeung,et al.  Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: the role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[98]  Stefanie Schuch,et al.  The costs of changing the representation of action: response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[99]  Elizabeth Duffy,et al.  Activation and behavior , 1962 .

[100]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[101]  T. Shallice,et al.  Task Switching : A PDP Model , 2001 .

[102]  C. Gallistel The Organization of Action: A New Synthesis , 1982 .

[103]  W. H. Teichner Recent studies of simple reaction time. , 1954, Psychological bulletin.

[104]  P. Rabbitt,et al.  Relationship between Rate of Preparation for, and Processing of, an Event Requiring a Choice Response , 1989 .

[105]  B. Hommel Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[106]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[107]  David E. Meyer,et al.  Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001 .

[108]  Michael Posner,et al.  Modulation by instruction , 1995, Nature.

[109]  G. Logan,et al.  On the Use of a Concurrent Memory Load to Measure Attention and Automaticity , 1979 .

[110]  Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching , 2003, Neuron.

[111]  R. Parasuraman The attentive brain , 1998 .

[112]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[113]  John W. Rohrbaugh,et al.  13 Sensory and Motor Aspects of the Contingent Negative Variation , 1983 .

[114]  H. Damasio,et al.  Dissociation Of Working Memory from Decision Making within the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[115]  B. Hommel The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation , 2000 .

[116]  R K Clifton,et al.  Heart-rate change as a component of the orienting response. , 1966, Psychological bulletin.

[117]  A. Osman,et al.  On the locus of speed-accuracy trade-off in reaction time: inferences from the lateralized readiness potential. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[118]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[119]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[120]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Task switching and multitask performance. , 2000 .

[121]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .

[122]  J. Richard Jennings,et al.  Speed-accuracy tradeoff functions in choice reaction time: Experimental designs and computational procedures , 1976 .

[123]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dissociable Neural Responses in Human Reward Systems , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[124]  Marc A. Sullivan,et al.  Coordinating information from perception and working memory , 1993 .

[125]  G. Logan,et al.  In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[126]  C. Degueldre,et al.  Regional brain activity during tasks devoted to the central executive of working memory. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[127]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[128]  M. Simonetta,et al.  Bereitschaftspotential in a simple movement or in a motor sequence starting with the same simple movement. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[129]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Cardiac cycle time effects on performance, phasic cardiac responses, and their intercorrelation in choice reaction time. , 1977, Psychophysiology.

[130]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  On the shift from anticipatory heart rate deceleration to acceleratory recovery: revisiting the role of response factors. , 1990, Psychophysiology.

[131]  Erik M. Altmann,et al.  Tasks of a feather flock together: Similarity effects in task switching , 2003, Memory & cognition.

[132]  R Gottsdanker,et al.  Verification of Donders' subtraction method. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[133]  C. Frith,et al.  The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention , 2001, Science.

[134]  U. Mayr Inhibition of action rules , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[135]  P. Lang,et al.  Anticipation and response set: cortical, cardiac, and electrodermal correlates. , 1979, Psychophysiology.

[136]  S. Nieuwenhuis,et al.  Mental fatigue and task control: planning and preparation. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[137]  Gerald E. Larson,et al.  SELF-REPORTED EFFORT VERSUS ACTUAL PERFORMANCE IN INFORMATION PROCESSING PARADIGMS , 1995 .

[138]  B. Postle,et al.  Dissociation of human caudate nucleus activity in spatial and nonspatial working memory: an event-related fMRI study. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[139]  M. Arbib 3 Interacting Schemas for Motor Control , 1980 .

[140]  The Generation of Motor Patterns , 1993 .

[141]  E Ruthruff,et al.  Switching between simple cognitive tasks: the interaction of top-down and bottom-up factors. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[142]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  The cardiac cycle time effect revisited: temporal dynamics of the central-vagal modulation of heart rate in human reaction time tasks. , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[143]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Action planning and the temporal binding of response codes , 1999 .

[144]  J. Lacey Somatic response patterning and stress : some revisions of activation theory , 1967 .

[145]  G. Logan Attention in character-classification tasks: Evidence for the automaticity of component stages. , 1978 .

[146]  R. Neiss,et al.  Ending arousal's reign of error: A reply to Anderson. , 1990 .

[147]  M. Brass,et al.  Decomposing Components of Task Preparation with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[148]  D E Kieras,et al.  A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. , 1997, Psychological review.

[149]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Effects of attention and stimulus probability on ERPs in a Go/Nogo task , 1993, Biological Psychology.

[150]  A. Greenwald,et al.  On doing two things at once: time sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[151]  E. Soetens Localizing sequential effects in serial choice reaction time with the information reduction procedure , 1998 .

[152]  J. Gregory Trafton,et al.  Memory for goals: an activation-based model , 2002, Cogn. Sci..

[153]  M. Moscovitch,et al.  Attention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing , 1994 .

[154]  Margaret Eberhardt I. A summary of some preliminary investigations of the deaf. , 1940 .

[155]  Eveline A. Crone,et al.  Cardiac concomitants of feedback processing , 2003, Biological Psychology.

[156]  J H Maunsell,et al.  The Brain's Visual World: Representation of Visual Targets in Cerebral Cortex , 1995, Science.

[157]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Residual costs in task switching: Testing the failure-to-engage hypothesis , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[158]  J. Jonides,et al.  Rehearsal in spatial working memory. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[159]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[160]  D. Rosenbaum Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[161]  M. Coles,et al.  Cardiac activity and information processing: the effects of stimulus significance, and detection and response requirements. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[162]  G Mulder,et al.  A psychophysiological study of the use of partial information in stimulus-response translation. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[163]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Predicting Success: Patterns of Cortical Activation and Deactivation Prior to Response Inhibition , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[164]  G. L. Freeman Discussion: 'Central' vs. 'peripheral' locus of set; a critique of the Mowrer, Rayman, and Bliss 'demonstration.' , 1940 .

[165]  L. Deecke,et al.  The Preparation and Execution of Self-Initiated and Externally-Triggered Movement: A Study of Event-Related fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.

[166]  S. H. Curry,et al.  Slow Potential Changes in the Human Brain , 1993, NATO ASI Series.

[167]  M. Coles,et al.  Handbook of cognitive psychophysiology : central and autonomic nervous system approaches , 1991 .

[168]  Alexa Riehle,et al.  Preparation for Action: one of the Key Functions of Motor Cortex , 2004 .

[169]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Neural Systems for Visual Orienting and Their Relationships to Spatial Working Memory , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[170]  S. Keele,et al.  Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[171]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Transition of Brain Activation from Frontal to Parietal Areas in Visuomotor Sequence Learning , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[172]  B C Lacey,et al.  Two-way communication between the heart and the brain. Significance of time within the cardiac cycle. , 1978, The American psychologist.

[173]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: I. Increases in Subcortical Structures and Cerebellum but Not in Nonvisual Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[174]  R. Hockey Stress and fatigue in human performance , 1984 .

[175]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Reaching and grasping. Parallel specification of visuomotor channels. , 1996 .

[176]  O. Mowrer Preparatory set (expectancy) - some methods of measurement. , 1940 .

[177]  A. Nambu,et al.  Activity of the Prefrontal Cortex on No-Go Decision and Motor Suppression , 1994 .

[178]  M. Brass,et al.  The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[179]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Varieties of attention , 1984 .

[180]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  fMRI Studies of Stroop Tasks Reveal Unique Roles of Anterior and Posterior Brain Systems in Attentional Selection , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[181]  M. Sarter,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: where top-down meets bottom-up , 2001, Brain Research Reviews.

[182]  G. L. Freeman The problem of set. , 1939 .

[183]  M M Mesulam,et al.  Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory , 1990, Annals of neurology.

[184]  H. J. Eysenck,et al.  Advances in psychophysiology: J.R. Jennings, P.K. Ackles & M.G.H. Coles (Eds) Vol.5 (1993).320 pp. £42.50 (hardback). ISBN 185302 191 1 , 1994 .

[185]  T. Bashore,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility Viewed from a Cognitiv E Psychophysiological Perspective , 1990 .

[186]  P. Obrist,et al.  Presidential Address, 1975. The cardiovascular-behavioral interaction--as it appears today. , 1976, Psychophysiology.

[187]  Tim Shallice,et al.  MULTIPLE LEVELS OF CONTROL PROCESSES , 1994 .

[188]  Brigitte Röder,et al.  Slow negative brain potentials as reflections of specific modular resources of cognition , 1997, Biological Psychology.

[189]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  Positive and negative effects of stimulus intensity in auditory reaction tasks: further studies on immediate arousal. , 1982, Acta psychologica.

[190]  G. Stelmach,et al.  Tutorials in Motor Behavior , 1980 .

[191]  P. Maquet,et al.  Orienting Attention to Locations in Perceptual Versus Mental Representations , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[192]  S. T. Klapp,et al.  Motor response programming during simple choice reaction time: The role of practice. , 1995 .

[193]  T. Shallice,et al.  A Multidisciplinary Approach to Anterior Attentional Functions a , 1995, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[194]  B. J. Casey,et al.  The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[195]  F. Freeman Experimental analysis of the writing movement. , 1914 .

[196]  T. Shallice From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure: Converging Operations: Specific Syndromes and Evidence from Normal Subjects , 1988 .

[197]  S. S. Stevens,et al.  Handbook of experimental psychology , 1951 .

[198]  L. Swanson Cerebral hemisphere regulation of motivated behavior 1 1 Published on the World Wide Web on 2 November 2000. , 2000, Brain Research.

[199]  C. Brunia,et al.  Anticipatory EMG responses of pericranial muscles in relation to heart rate during a warned simple reaction time task. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[200]  Slow Potentials in Anticipatory Behavior , 2004 .

[201]  A. Nobre,et al.  Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[202]  D. Wright,et al.  Motor programming during practice conditions high and low in contextual interference. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[203]  A. Kok On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[204]  E. Soetens,et al.  Covert signs of expectancy in serial reaction time tasks revealed by event-related potentials , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[205]  B. Rockstroh,et al.  Slow potentials of the cerebral cortex and behavior. , 1990, Physiological reviews.

[206]  A. Glenberg,et al.  What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[207]  R. Näätänen,et al.  The relationship between the contingent negative variation and the reaction time under prolonged experimental conditions. , 1974, Biological psychology.

[208]  Thomas Elbert,et al.  SLOW CORTICAL POTENTIALS REFLECT THE REGULATION OF CORTICAL EXCITABILITY , 1993 .

[209]  B. Postle,et al.  “ What ” — Then — “ Where ” in Visual Working Memory : An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2000 .

[210]  Jos J. Adam,et al.  The up–right/down–left advantage for vertically oriented stimuli and horizontally oriented responses: A dual-strategy hypothesis. , 1998 .

[211]  M. Mesulam,et al.  From sensation to cognition. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[212]  D. Zald,et al.  Anatomy and function of the orbital frontal cortex, I: anatomy, neurocircuitry; and obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1996, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[213]  Cameron S Carter,et al.  Prefrontal Cortex Guides Context-Appropriate Responding during Language Production , 2004, Neuron.

[214]  Anders Kjellberg,et al.  Orienting Activity in Two-Stimulus Paradigms as Reflected in Heart Rate , 2021, The Orienting Reflex in Humans.

[215]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1981 .

[216]  T. Goschke Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching , 2000 .

[217]  Donald A. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action , 1986 .

[218]  W. R. Garner The Processing of Information and Structure , 1974 .

[219]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  Preparing the heart, eye, and brain: foreperiod length effects in a nonaging paradigm. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[220]  I. Koch,et al.  The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[221]  Y. Okamoto,et al.  Brain activity during expectancy of emotional stimuli: an fMRI study , 2003, Neuroreport.

[222]  Hugo D. Critchley,et al.  Brain activity relating to the contingent negative variation: an fMRI investigation , 2004, NeuroImage.

[223]  R. Neiss Reconceptualizing arousal: psychobiological states in motor performance. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.

[224]  C. Wientjes,et al.  The relation between event-related brain potential, heart rate, and blood pressure responses in an S1-S2 paradigm , 1995, Biological Psychology.

[225]  E. Donchin,et al.  Psychophysiology : systems, processes, and applications , 1987 .

[226]  R. Passingham,et al.  Multiple Movement Representations in the Human Brain: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[227]  L. Putnam Great expectations: Anticipatory responses of the heart and brain. , 1990 .

[228]  F. Vidal,et al.  Programming response duration in a precueing reaction time paradigm. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.

[229]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[230]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Locus of the effect of the number of alternative responses : Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential , 1998 .

[231]  T Hasbroucq,et al.  Cortico-spinal inhibition reflects time but not event preparation: neural mechanisms of preparation dissociated by transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[232]  Gijsbertus Mulder,et al.  Psychophysiology of cardiovascular control : models, methods, and data , 1985 .

[233]  W. Wundt,et al.  Grundzüge der physiologischen psyhcologie , 1893 .

[234]  Robert Gottsdanker,et al.  Effort of Preparation and Age , 1984, Perceptual and motor skills.

[235]  B. Hommel,et al.  Preparing for perception and action (I): The role of grouping in the response-cuing paradigm , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[236]  R. Schmidt 8 On the Theoretical Status of Time in Motor Program Representations , 1980 .

[237]  J. Requin,et al.  Movement Amplitude, Pointing Accuracy and Choice-Reaction Time , 1976 .

[238]  N Birbaumer,et al.  Mapping P300 waves onto inhibition: Go/No-Go discrimination. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[239]  S. A. Wallace Perspectives on the coordination of movement , 1989 .

[240]  M. Brass,et al.  Advance preparation and stimulus-induced interference in cued task switching: further insights from BOLD fMRI , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[241]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  Locus of the effect of temporal preparation: evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. , 2003, Psychophysiology.

[242]  K. Carlsson,et al.  Tickling Expectations: Neural Processing in Anticipation of a Sensory Stimulus , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[243]  R. Lazarus,et al.  Some parameters of heart rate change: perceptual versus motor task requirements, noxiousness, and uncertainty. , 1970, Psychophysiology.

[244]  E. Boring A History of Experimental Psychology. , 1930 .

[245]  S. Steinhauer,et al.  The Pupillary Response in Cognitive Psychophysiology and Schizophrenia a , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[246]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[247]  D. Navon Resources—a theoretical soup stone? , 1984 .

[248]  Maurits W. van der Molen,et al.  Cardiac timing and the central regulation of action , 2002, Psychological research.

[249]  E. Rolls,et al.  Attention and working memory: a dynamical model of neuronal activity in the prefrontal cortex , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[250]  H. Heuer,et al.  Perspectives on Perception and Action , 1989 .

[251]  Wayne A. Wickelgren,et al.  Speed-accuracy tradeoff and information processing dynamics , 1977 .

[252]  I. Jentzsch,et al.  Beneficial effects of ambiguous precues: parallel motor preparation or reduced premotoric processing time? , 2004, Psychophysiology.

[253]  Sigmund Exner,et al.  Experimentelle Untersuchung der einfachsten psychischen Processe , 1875, Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.

[254]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  Wisconsin Card Sorting in adolescents: analysis of performance, response times and heart rate. , 2000, Acta psychologica.

[255]  A. Nambu,et al.  No-go activity in the frontal association cortex of human subjects , 1993, Neuroscience Research.

[256]  A. Sanford,et al.  Slow potential correlates of preparatory set. , 1974, Biological psychology.

[257]  R. Passingham,et al.  The prefrontal cortex: response selection or maintenance within working memory? , 2000, 5th IEEE EMBS International Summer School on Biomedical Imaging, 2002..

[258]  O. Neumann Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention , 1987 .

[259]  David E. Kieras,et al.  A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 2. Accounts of psychological refractory-period phenomena. , 1997 .

[260]  M. Jeannerod The cognitive neuroscience of action , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[261]  A. Mecklinger,et al.  Event-related potentials reveal topographical and temporal distinct neuronal activation patterns for spatial and object working memory. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[262]  M. Posner Chronometric explorations of mind : the third Paul M. Fitts lectures, delivered at the University of Michigan, September 1976 , 1978 .

[263]  W. McD.,et al.  Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie Principles of Physiological Psychology , 1905, Nature.

[264]  A. Allport Attention and control: have we been asking the wrong questions? A critical review of twenty-five years , 1993 .

[265]  C. Brunia Neural aspects of anticipatory behavior. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[266]  M Eimer,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: evidence from psychophysiological studies. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[267]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[268]  Tim Shallice,et al.  Supervisory control of action and thought selection. , 1993 .

[269]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Discrete and continuous models of human information processing: theoretical distinctions and empirical results. , 1988, Acta psychologica.

[270]  H. Woodrow The measurement of attention , 1914 .