Preparation for speeded action as a psychophysiological concept.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[2] M. Mon-Williams,et al. Motor Control and Learning , 2006 .
[3] H. Critchley,et al. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[4] E Berendsen,et al. Goal neglect and inhibitory limitations: dissociable causes of interference effects in conflict situations. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[5] N. Meiran,et al. Component Processes in Task Switching , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.
[6] H Pashler,et al. Shifting visual attention and selecting motor responses: distinct attentional mechanisms. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] Jörn Diedrichsen,et al. Bimanual interference associated with the selection of target locations. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] S. Monsell,et al. Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .
[9] J. Jennings,et al. Recall, recognition, and rate: memory and the heart. , 1980, Psychophysiology.
[10] A. Osman,et al. Mechanisms of speed–accuracy tradeoff: evidence from covert motor processes , 2000, Biological Psychology.
[11] W. N. Schoenfeld,et al. Principles of Psychology , 2007 .
[12] L. Lange. Neue Experimente uber den Vorgang der einfachen Reaction auf Sinneseindrucke , 1888 .
[13] David A. Rosenbaum,et al. Reaching and Grasping , 1990 .
[14] F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Computations underlying the execution of movement: a biological perspective. , 1991, Science.
[15] Odmar Neumann. Theories of attention. , 1996 .
[16] G. E. Alexander,et al. Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.
[17] G D Logan,et al. Constraints on strategy construction in a speeded discrimination task. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[18] 津田 敏雄,et al. Contingent negative variation , 1984 .
[19] C. Brunia,et al. Waiting in readiness: gating in attention and motor preparation. , 1993, Psychophysiology.
[20] E. Soetens,et al. Expectancy or Automatic Facilitation? Separating Sequential Effects in Two-Choice Reaction Time , 1985 .
[21] H. Heuer. Dual-task performance. , 1996 .
[22] J. Jolles,et al. Rapid visuomotor preparation in the human brain: a functional MRI study. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[23] D. Heslenfeld,et al. Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation: electrophysiological evidence. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[24] B. Rockstroh. Slow cortical potentials and behavior , 1989 .
[25] J. Mink. THE BASAL GANGLIA: FOCUSED SELECTION AND INHIBITION OF COMPETING MOTOR PROGRAMS , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.
[26] D. A. Taylor,et al. The cuing and priming of cognitive operations. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[27] Richard Trumbull,et al. Psychological stress : issues in research , 1967 .
[28] T. Robbins,et al. The prefrontal cortex: Executive and cognitive functions. , 1998 .
[29] Ernst Fernando Lopes Da Silva Niedermeyer,et al. Electroencephalography, basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields , 1982 .
[30] R. Näätänen. Attention and brain function , 1992 .
[31] Ritske de Jong,et al. Strategical determinants of compatibility effects with task uncertainty , 1995 .
[32] G. Sperling,et al. Episodic theory of the dynamics of spatial attention. , 1995 .
[33] Ray Johnson,et al. Event-related brain potentials : basic issues and applications , 1990 .
[34] N. Meiran. Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.
[35] Stephen M. Rao,et al. Motor Sequence Complexity and Performing Hand Produce Differential Patterns of Hemispheric Lateralization , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[36] Edward Awh,et al. Top-down control over biased competition during covert spatial orienting. , 2003 .
[38] P E Roland,et al. Somatotopical tuning of postcentral gyrus during focal attention in man. A regional cerebral blood flow study. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.
[39] G. Logan,et al. Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[40] Hisae Gemba,et al. Potential related to no-go reaction in go/no-go hand movement with discrimination between tone stimuli of different frequencies in the monkey , 1990, Brain Research.
[41] Oswald Kuelpe. The problem of attention , 1902 .
[42] D I Boomsma,et al. Does the heart know what the eye sees? A cardiac/pupillometric analysis of motor preparation and response execution. , 1989, Psychophysiology.
[43] A. Baddeley,et al. Attention : selection, awareness, and control : a tribute to Donald Broadbent , 1996 .
[44] P. Jolicoeur,et al. Can blindness to response-compatible stimuli be observed in the absence of a response? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[45] D. Kahneman,et al. Attention and Effort , 1973 .
[46] M. Mesulam. A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect , 1981, Annals of neurology.
[47] J R Anderson,et al. Task preparation and task repetition: two-component model of task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[48] R. Kliegl,et al. Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[49] G. Band,et al. Speed-accuracy modulation in case of conflict: the roles of activation and inhibition , 2003, Psychological research.
[50] J. Müsseler,et al. Time course of the blindness to response-compatible stimuli. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[51] Gordon D Logan,et al. Working memory, task switching, and executive control in the task span procedure. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[52] Rolf Verleger,et al. Posterior and anterior contribution of hand-movement preparation to late CNV. , 2000 .
[53] P J Beek,et al. Dynamical substructure of coordinated rhythmic movements. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[54] J. Fuster. The Prefrontal Cortex , 1997 .
[55] Herbert Heuer,et al. Processes of task-set reconfiguration: switching operations and implementation operations. , 2002, Acta psychologica.
[56] J. Ford,et al. ERPs to response production and inhibition. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[57] James J. Gibson,et al. A critical review of the concept of set in contemporary experimental psychology. , 1941 .
[58] H. Pashler. The Psychology of Attention , 1997 .
[59] A. Damasio. Descartes' error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. avon books , 1994 .
[60] A. Sanders. Towards a model of stress and human performance. , 1983, Acta psychologica.
[61] P. Goldman-Rakic,et al. Activation of human prefrontal cortex during spatial and nonspatial working memory tasks measured by functional MRI. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.
[62] C. V. D. Heuvel,et al. Intentional and unintentional contributions of nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods. , 2001 .
[63] R. Forget,et al. The modulation of somatosensory evoked potentials during the foreperiod of a forewarned reaction time task. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[64] S. Hackley,et al. Preparation for Donders' type B and C reaction tasks. , 1990, Acta psychologica.
[65] K. Willmes,et al. On the Functional Neuroanatomy of Intrinsic and Phasic Alertness , 2001, NeuroImage.
[66] A. Thierry,et al. Motor and Cognitive Functions of the Prefrontal Cortex , 1994, Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences.
[67] Heikki Lyytinen,et al. Physiology of preparation: Brain slow waves, heart rate, and respiration preceding triggering in rifle shooting. , 1992 .
[68] P McLeod,et al. Does Probe RT Measure Central Processing Demand? , 1978, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[69] T. Carr,et al. Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language , 1994 .
[70] Natalie A. Phillips,et al. Behavioural and electrophysiological measures of task switching during single and mixed-task conditions , 2006, Biological Psychology.
[71] M. Corbetta. Functional anatomy of visual attention in the human brain: Studies with positron emission tomography. , 1998 .
[72] David W. Martin,et al. Processing demands of encoding: What does secondary task performance reflect? , 1980 .
[73] A. Nobre,et al. Orienting Attention to Locations in Internal Representations , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[74] J. Beatty. Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. , 1982 .
[75] E. Zarahn,et al. The Role of Prefrontal Cortex in Sensory Memory and Motor Preparation: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2000, NeuroImage.
[76] M. S. Mayzner,et al. Cognition And Reality , 1976 .
[77] James H. Abbs,et al. Motor Coordination for Functional Human Behaviors: Perspectives From A Speech Motor Data Base , 1989 .
[78] R. Spinks. The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology, and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobe, 3rd ed. , 2000 .
[79] Bruce D. McCandliss,et al. Testing the Efficiency and Independence of Attentional Networks , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[80] J. Mazziotta,et al. Brain Mapping: The Methods , 2002 .
[81] G. Schwartz,et al. Consciousness and Self-Regulation , 1976 .
[82] M. Raichle,et al. Blood flow changes in human somatosensory cortex during anticipated stimulation , 1995, Nature.
[83] Neurophysiology and Psychophysiology , 2021 .
[84] S. Los,et al. Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[85] C. Brunia. Movement and stimulus preceding negativity , 1988, Biological Psychology.
[86] S. Hackley,et al. Which stages of processing are speeded by a warning signal? , 2003, Biological Psychology.
[87] M. Coltheart,et al. Electrophysiological correlates of anticipatory and poststimulus components of task switching. , 2003, Psychophysiology.
[88] E. Donchin,et al. Detecting early communication: Using measures of movement-related potentials to illuminate human information processing , 1988, Biological Psychology.
[89] J. Cohen,et al. Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. , 2000, Science.
[90] L. Karlin. Reaction time as a function of foreperiod duration and variability. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.
[91] J. Pratt,et al. Dissociating visual attention and effector selection in spatial precuing tasks. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[92] M. W. Molen,et al. A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm , 2001, Biological Psychology.
[93] H. Pashler. Dual-task interference and elementary mental mechanisms , 1993 .
[94] Cornelis H. M. Brunia,et al. Reflexes as a tool: A window in the central nervous system , 1988 .
[95] S Monsell,et al. Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task? , 2000, Psychological research.
[96] Keith J. Holyoak,et al. Structure and Functions of the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 1996 .
[97] N. Yeung,et al. Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: the role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[98] Stefanie Schuch,et al. The costs of changing the representation of action: response repetition and response-response compatibility in dual tasks. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[99] Elizabeth Duffy,et al. Activation and behavior , 1962 .
[100] W. James,et al. The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .
[101] T. Shallice,et al. Task Switching : A PDP Model , 2001 .
[102] C. Gallistel. The Organization of Action: A New Synthesis , 1982 .
[103] W. H. Teichner. Recent studies of simple reaction time. , 1954, Psychological bulletin.
[104] P. Rabbitt,et al. Relationship between Rate of Preparation for, and Processing of, an Event Requiring a Choice Response , 1989 .
[105] B. Hommel. Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[106] C. Eriksen,et al. Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[107] David E. Meyer,et al. Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001 .
[108] Michael Posner,et al. Modulation by instruction , 1995, Nature.
[109] G. Logan,et al. On the Use of a Concurrent Memory Load to Measure Attention and Automaticity , 1979 .
[110] Jeremy R. Reynolds,et al. Neural Mechanisms of Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching , 2003, Neuron.
[111] R. Parasuraman. The attentive brain , 1998 .
[112] M. Posner,et al. The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.
[113] John W. Rohrbaugh,et al. 13 Sensory and Motor Aspects of the Contingent Negative Variation , 1983 .
[114] H. Damasio,et al. Dissociation Of Working Memory from Decision Making within the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[115] B. Hommel. The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation , 2000 .
[116] R K Clifton,et al. Heart-rate change as a component of the orienting response. , 1966, Psychological bulletin.
[117] A. Osman,et al. On the locus of speed-accuracy trade-off in reaction time: inferences from the lateralized readiness potential. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[118] M. Raichle,et al. Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[119] R. Duncan Luce,et al. Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .
[120] Richard B. Ivry,et al. Task switching and multitask performance. , 2000 .
[121] Robert W. Proctor,et al. Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .
[122] J. Richard Jennings,et al. Speed-accuracy tradeoff functions in choice reaction time: Experimental designs and computational procedures , 1976 .
[123] Karl J. Friston,et al. Dissociable Neural Responses in Human Reward Systems , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[124] Marc A. Sullivan,et al. Coordinating information from perception and working memory , 1993 .
[125] G. Logan,et al. In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[126] C. Degueldre,et al. Regional brain activity during tasks devoted to the central executive of working memory. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[127] John R. Anderson,et al. The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex in task switching. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[128] M. Simonetta,et al. Bereitschaftspotential in a simple movement or in a motor sequence starting with the same simple movement. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[129] C. C. Wood,et al. Cardiac cycle time effects on performance, phasic cardiac responses, and their intercorrelation in choice reaction time. , 1977, Psychophysiology.
[130] M. W. van der Molen,et al. On the shift from anticipatory heart rate deceleration to acceleratory recovery: revisiting the role of response factors. , 1990, Psychophysiology.
[131] Erik M. Altmann,et al. Tasks of a feather flock together: Similarity effects in task switching , 2003, Memory & cognition.
[132] R Gottsdanker,et al. Verification of Donders' subtraction method. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[133] C. Frith,et al. The Role of Working Memory in Visual Selective Attention , 2001, Science.
[134] U. Mayr. Inhibition of action rules , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[135] P. Lang,et al. Anticipation and response set: cortical, cardiac, and electrodermal correlates. , 1979, Psychophysiology.
[136] S. Nieuwenhuis,et al. Mental fatigue and task control: planning and preparation. , 2000, Psychophysiology.
[137] Gerald E. Larson,et al. SELF-REPORTED EFFORT VERSUS ACTUAL PERFORMANCE IN INFORMATION PROCESSING PARADIGMS , 1995 .
[138] B. Postle,et al. Dissociation of human caudate nucleus activity in spatial and nonspatial working memory: an event-related fMRI study. , 1999, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[139] M. Arbib. 3 Interacting Schemas for Motor Control , 1980 .
[140] The Generation of Motor Patterns , 1993 .
[141] E Ruthruff,et al. Switching between simple cognitive tasks: the interaction of top-down and bottom-up factors. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[142] M. W. van der Molen,et al. The cardiac cycle time effect revisited: temporal dynamics of the central-vagal modulation of heart rate in human reaction time tasks. , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[143] Bernhard Hommel,et al. Action planning and the temporal binding of response codes , 1999 .
[144] J. Lacey. Somatic response patterning and stress : some revisions of activation theory , 1967 .
[145] G. Logan. Attention in character-classification tasks: Evidence for the automaticity of component stages. , 1978 .
[146] R. Neiss,et al. Ending arousal's reign of error: A reply to Anderson. , 1990 .
[147] M. Brass,et al. Decomposing Components of Task Preparation with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[148] D E Kieras,et al. A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. , 1997, Psychological review.
[149] Martin Eimer,et al. Effects of attention and stimulus probability on ERPs in a Go/Nogo task , 1993, Biological Psychology.
[150] A. Greenwald,et al. On doing two things at once: time sharing as a function of ideomotor compatibility. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.
[151] E. Soetens. Localizing sequential effects in serial choice reaction time with the information reduction procedure , 1998 .
[152] J. Gregory Trafton,et al. Memory for goals: an activation-based model , 2002, Cogn. Sci..
[153] M. Moscovitch,et al. Attention and Performance 15: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing , 1994 .
[154] Margaret Eberhardt. I. A summary of some preliminary investigations of the deaf. , 1940 .
[155] Eveline A. Crone,et al. Cardiac concomitants of feedback processing , 2003, Biological Psychology.
[156] J H Maunsell,et al. The Brain's Visual World: Representation of Visual Targets in Cerebral Cortex , 1995, Science.
[157] Stephen Monsell,et al. Residual costs in task switching: Testing the failure-to-engage hypothesis , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[158] J. Jonides,et al. Rehearsal in spatial working memory. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[159] G. E. Alexander,et al. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.
[160] D. Rosenbaum. Human movement initiation: specification of arm, direction, and extent. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[161] M. Coles,et al. Cardiac activity and information processing: the effects of stimulus significance, and detection and response requirements. , 1975, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[162] G Mulder,et al. A psychophysiological study of the use of partial information in stimulus-response translation. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[163] John J. Foxe,et al. Predicting Success: Patterns of Cortical Activation and Deactivation Prior to Response Inhibition , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[164] G. L. Freeman. Discussion: 'Central' vs. 'peripheral' locus of set; a critique of the Mowrer, Rayman, and Bliss 'demonstration.' , 1940 .
[165] L. Deecke,et al. The Preparation and Execution of Self-Initiated and Externally-Triggered Movement: A Study of Event-Related fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.
[166] S. H. Curry,et al. Slow Potential Changes in the Human Brain , 1993, NATO ASI Series.
[167] M. Coles,et al. Handbook of cognitive psychophysiology : central and autonomic nervous system approaches , 1991 .
[168] Alexa Riehle,et al. Preparation for Action: one of the Key Functions of Motor Cortex , 2004 .
[169] M. Corbetta,et al. Neural Systems for Visual Orienting and Their Relationships to Spatial Working Memory , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[170] S. Keele,et al. Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[171] O. Hikosaka,et al. Transition of Brain Activation from Frontal to Parietal Areas in Visuomotor Sequence Learning , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[172] B C Lacey,et al. Two-way communication between the heart and the brain. Significance of time within the cardiac cycle. , 1978, The American psychologist.
[173] M. Corbetta,et al. Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: I. Increases in Subcortical Structures and Cerebellum but Not in Nonvisual Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[174] R. Hockey. Stress and fatigue in human performance , 1984 .
[175] M. Jeannerod,et al. Reaching and grasping. Parallel specification of visuomotor channels. , 1996 .
[176] O. Mowrer. Preparatory set (expectancy) - some methods of measurement. , 1940 .
[177] A. Nambu,et al. Activity of the Prefrontal Cortex on No-Go Decision and Motor Suppression , 1994 .
[178] M. Brass,et al. The role of the frontal cortex in task preparation. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.
[179] Raja Parasuraman,et al. Varieties of attention , 1984 .
[180] Arthur F. Kramer,et al. fMRI Studies of Stroop Tasks Reveal Unique Roles of Anterior and Posterior Brain Systems in Attentional Selection , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[181] M. Sarter,et al. The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: where top-down meets bottom-up , 2001, Brain Research Reviews.
[182] G. L. Freeman. The problem of set. , 1939 .
[183] M M Mesulam,et al. Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory , 1990, Annals of neurology.
[184] H. J. Eysenck,et al. Advances in psychophysiology: J.R. Jennings, P.K. Ackles & M.G.H. Coles (Eds) Vol.5 (1993).320 pp. £42.50 (hardback). ISBN 185302 191 1 , 1994 .
[185] T. Bashore,et al. Stimulus-Response Compatibility Viewed from a Cognitiv E Psychophysiological Perspective , 1990 .
[186] P. Obrist,et al. Presidential Address, 1975. The cardiovascular-behavioral interaction--as it appears today. , 1976, Psychophysiology.
[187] Tim Shallice,et al. MULTIPLE LEVELS OF CONTROL PROCESSES , 1994 .
[188] Brigitte Röder,et al. Slow negative brain potentials as reflections of specific modular resources of cognition , 1997, Biological Psychology.
[189] M. W. van der Molen,et al. Positive and negative effects of stimulus intensity in auditory reaction tasks: further studies on immediate arousal. , 1982, Acta psychologica.
[190] G. Stelmach,et al. Tutorials in Motor Behavior , 1980 .
[191] P. Maquet,et al. Orienting Attention to Locations in Perceptual Versus Mental Representations , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[192] S. T. Klapp,et al. Motor response programming during simple choice reaction time: The role of practice. , 1995 .
[193] T. Shallice,et al. A Multidisciplinary Approach to Anterior Attentional Functions a , 1995, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[194] B. J. Casey,et al. The Effect of Preceding Context on Inhibition: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.
[195] F. Freeman. Experimental analysis of the writing movement. , 1914 .
[196] T. Shallice. From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure: Converging Operations: Specific Syndromes and Evidence from Normal Subjects , 1988 .
[197] S. S. Stevens,et al. Handbook of experimental psychology , 1951 .
[198] L. Swanson. Cerebral hemisphere regulation of motivated behavior 1 1 Published on the World Wide Web on 2 November 2000. , 2000, Brain Research.
[199] C. Brunia,et al. Anticipatory EMG responses of pericranial muscles in relation to heart rate during a warned simple reaction time task. , 1996, Psychophysiology.
[200] Slow Potentials in Anticipatory Behavior , 2004 .
[201] A. Nobre,et al. Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[202] D. Wright,et al. Motor programming during practice conditions high and low in contextual interference. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[203] A. Kok. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[204] E. Soetens,et al. Covert signs of expectancy in serial reaction time tasks revealed by event-related potentials , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[205] B. Rockstroh,et al. Slow potentials of the cerebral cortex and behavior. , 1990, Physiological reviews.
[206] A. Glenberg,et al. What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[207] R. Näätänen,et al. The relationship between the contingent negative variation and the reaction time under prolonged experimental conditions. , 1974, Biological psychology.
[208] Thomas Elbert,et al. SLOW CORTICAL POTENTIALS REFLECT THE REGULATION OF CORTICAL EXCITABILITY , 1993 .
[209] B. Postle,et al. “ What ” — Then — “ Where ” in Visual Working Memory : An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2000 .
[210] Jos J. Adam,et al. The up–right/down–left advantage for vertically oriented stimuli and horizontally oriented responses: A dual-strategy hypothesis. , 1998 .
[211] M. Mesulam,et al. From sensation to cognition. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[212] D. Zald,et al. Anatomy and function of the orbital frontal cortex, I: anatomy, neurocircuitry; and obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 1996, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.
[213] Cameron S Carter,et al. Prefrontal Cortex Guides Context-Appropriate Responding during Language Production , 2004, Neuron.
[214] Anders Kjellberg,et al. Orienting Activity in Two-Stimulus Paradigms as Reflected in Heart Rate , 2021, The Orienting Reflex in Humans.
[215] R. Näätänen,et al. Foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1981 .
[216] T. Goschke. Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching , 2000 .
[217] Donald A. Norman,et al. Attention to Action , 1986 .
[218] W. R. Garner. The Processing of Information and Structure , 1974 .
[219] M. W. van der Molen,et al. Preparing the heart, eye, and brain: foreperiod length effects in a nonaging paradigm. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[220] I. Koch,et al. The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[221] Y. Okamoto,et al. Brain activity during expectancy of emotional stimuli: an fMRI study , 2003, Neuroreport.
[222] Hugo D. Critchley,et al. Brain activity relating to the contingent negative variation: an fMRI investigation , 2004, NeuroImage.
[223] R. Neiss. Reconceptualizing arousal: psychobiological states in motor performance. , 1988, Psychological bulletin.
[224] C. Wientjes,et al. The relation between event-related brain potential, heart rate, and blood pressure responses in an S1-S2 paradigm , 1995, Biological Psychology.
[225] E. Donchin,et al. Psychophysiology : systems, processes, and applications , 1987 .
[226] R. Passingham,et al. Multiple Movement Representations in the Human Brain: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[227] L. Putnam. Great expectations: Anticipatory responses of the heart and brain. , 1990 .
[228] F. Vidal,et al. Programming response duration in a precueing reaction time paradigm. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.
[229] E. Donchin,et al. Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[230] Jeff Miller,et al. Locus of the effect of the number of alternative responses : Evidence from the lateralized readiness potential , 1998 .
[231] T Hasbroucq,et al. Cortico-spinal inhibition reflects time but not event preparation: neural mechanisms of preparation dissociated by transcranial magnetic stimulation. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[232] Gijsbertus Mulder,et al. Psychophysiology of cardiovascular control : models, methods, and data , 1985 .
[233] W. Wundt,et al. Grundzüge der physiologischen psyhcologie , 1893 .
[234] Robert Gottsdanker,et al. Effort of Preparation and Age , 1984, Perceptual and motor skills.
[235] B. Hommel,et al. Preparing for perception and action (I): The role of grouping in the response-cuing paradigm , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.
[236] R. Schmidt. 8 On the Theoretical Status of Time in Motor Program Representations , 1980 .
[237] J. Requin,et al. Movement Amplitude, Pointing Accuracy and Choice-Reaction Time , 1976 .
[238] N Birbaumer,et al. Mapping P300 waves onto inhibition: Go/No-Go discrimination. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[239] S. A. Wallace. Perspectives on the coordination of movement , 1989 .
[240] M. Brass,et al. Advance preparation and stimulus-induced interference in cued task switching: further insights from BOLD fMRI , 2005, Neuropsychologia.
[241] Rolf Ulrich,et al. Locus of the effect of temporal preparation: evidence from the lateralized readiness potential. , 2003, Psychophysiology.
[242] K. Carlsson,et al. Tickling Expectations: Neural Processing in Anticipation of a Sensory Stimulus , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[243] R. Lazarus,et al. Some parameters of heart rate change: perceptual versus motor task requirements, noxiousness, and uncertainty. , 1970, Psychophysiology.
[244] E. Boring. A History of Experimental Psychology. , 1930 .
[245] S. Steinhauer,et al. The Pupillary Response in Cognitive Psychophysiology and Schizophrenia a , 1992, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[246] R. Cabeza,et al. Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[247] D. Navon. Resources—a theoretical soup stone? , 1984 .
[248] Maurits W. van der Molen,et al. Cardiac timing and the central regulation of action , 2002, Psychological research.
[249] E. Rolls,et al. Attention and working memory: a dynamical model of neuronal activity in the prefrontal cortex , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.
[250] H. Heuer,et al. Perspectives on Perception and Action , 1989 .
[251] Wayne A. Wickelgren,et al. Speed-accuracy tradeoff and information processing dynamics , 1977 .
[252] I. Jentzsch,et al. Beneficial effects of ambiguous precues: parallel motor preparation or reduced premotoric processing time? , 2004, Psychophysiology.
[253] Sigmund Exner,et al. Experimentelle Untersuchung der einfachsten psychischen Processe , 1875, Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.
[254] M. W. van der Molen,et al. Wisconsin Card Sorting in adolescents: analysis of performance, response times and heart rate. , 2000, Acta psychologica.
[255] A. Nambu,et al. No-go activity in the frontal association cortex of human subjects , 1993, Neuroscience Research.
[256] A. Sanford,et al. Slow potential correlates of preparatory set. , 1974, Biological psychology.
[257] R. Passingham,et al. The prefrontal cortex: response selection or maintenance within working memory? , 2000, 5th IEEE EMBS International Summer School on Biomedical Imaging, 2002..
[258] O. Neumann. Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention , 1987 .
[259] David E. Kieras,et al. A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 2. Accounts of psychological refractory-period phenomena. , 1997 .
[260] M. Jeannerod. The cognitive neuroscience of action , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[261] A. Mecklinger,et al. Event-related potentials reveal topographical and temporal distinct neuronal activation patterns for spatial and object working memory. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[262] M. Posner. Chronometric explorations of mind : the third Paul M. Fitts lectures, delivered at the University of Michigan, September 1976 , 1978 .
[263] W. McD.,et al. Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie Principles of Physiological Psychology , 1905, Nature.
[264] A. Allport. Attention and control: have we been asking the wrong questions? A critical review of twenty-five years , 1993 .
[265] C. Brunia. Neural aspects of anticipatory behavior. , 1999, Acta psychologica.
[266] M Eimer,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: evidence from psychophysiological studies. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[267] M. Corbetta,et al. Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[268] Tim Shallice,et al. Supervisory control of action and thought selection. , 1993 .
[269] Jeff Miller,et al. Discrete and continuous models of human information processing: theoretical distinctions and empirical results. , 1988, Acta psychologica.
[270] H. Woodrow. The measurement of attention , 1914 .