What Do You Mean, No? Toddlers’ Comprehension of Logical “No” and “Not”

ABSTRACT For adults, “no” and “not” change the truth-value of sentences they compose with. To investigate children’s emerging understanding of these words, an experimenter hid a ball in a bucket or a truck, then gave an affirmative or negative clue (Experiment 1: “It’s not in the bucket”; Experiment 2: “Is it in the bucket?”; “No, it’s not”). Replicating Austin, Theakston, Lieven, & Tomasello (2014), children only understood logical “no” and “not” after age two, long after they say “no” but around the time they say “not” and use both words to deny statements. To investigate whether this simply reflects improving inhibitory control, in Experiment 3 we showed children that one container did or did not hold the ball. Twenty-month-olds now succeeded. We discuss two possible factors limiting learning both “no” and “not”—a purely linguistic difficulty learning the labels, and the possibility that negation is unavailable to thought before age two.

[1]  Kyung J. Kim Development of the Concept of Truth-Functional Negation. , 1985 .

[2]  Jesse Snedeker,et al.  Starting Over , 2007, Psychological science.

[3]  R. Pea,et al.  Origins of verbal logic: spontaneous denials by two- and three-year olds , 1982, Journal of Child Language.

[4]  Anna L. Theakston,et al.  Young children's understanding of denial. , 2014, Developmental psychology.

[5]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Experiential simulations of negated text information , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  Anna L. Theakston,et al.  What part of no do children not understand? A usage-based account of multiword negation , 2007, Journal of Child Language.

[7]  D. Barr,et al.  Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. , 2013, Journal of memory and language.

[8]  L. Bloom Language Development: Form and Function in Emerging Grammars , 1970 .

[9]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Wordbank: an open repository for developmental vocabulary data* , 2016, Journal of Child Language.

[10]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  The role of context in young children's comprehension of negation , 2014 .

[11]  David Barner,et al.  Why is number word learning hard? Evidence from bilingual learners , 2015, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  K. Miller Executive functions. , 2005, Pediatric annals.

[13]  H Wimmer,et al.  On the origins of denial negation , 1993, Journal of Child Language.

[14]  Jill de Villiers,et al.  Some facts one simply cannot deny , 1975, Journal of Child Language.

[15]  L. Fenson,et al.  Lexical development norms for young children , 1996 .

[16]  Susan Carey,et al.  The emergence of reasoning by the disjunctive syllogism in early childhood , 2016, Cognition.

[17]  R. Pea The development of negation in early child language , 1978 .

[18]  R. Pea LOGIC IN EARLY CHILD LANGUAGE * , 1980 .

[19]  P. Wason The contexts of plausible denial , 1965 .

[20]  J. Steven Reznick,et al.  Short-form versions of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories , 2000, Applied Psycholinguistics.

[21]  J. Snedeker,et al.  Disentangling the effects of cognitive development and linguistic expertise: A longitudinal study of the acquisition of English in internationally-adopted children , 2012, Cognitive Psychology.

[22]  Laurence R. Horn A Natural History of Negation , 1989 .

[23]  S. Choi,et al.  The semantic development of negation: a cross-linguistic longitudinal study , 1988, Journal of Child Language.

[24]  Michèle Guidetti Yes or no? How young French children combine gestures and speech to agree and refuse , 2005, Journal of Child Language.

[25]  George A. Miller,et al.  The Social foundations of language and thought : essays in honor of Jerome S. Bruner , 1980 .

[26]  Peter Lloyd,et al.  On a method of eliciting true/false judgments from young children , 1976, Journal of Child Language.

[27]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Processing negated sentences with contradictory predicates: Is a door that is not open mentally closed? , 2006 .