An object-color space.

Putting aside metaphorical meanings of the term, color space is understood as a vector space, where lights having the same color (i.e., subjectively indistinguishable) are represented as a point. The CIE 1931 color space, empirically based on trichromatic color measurements, is a classical example. Its derivatives, such as CIELAB and sRGB, have been successfully used in many applications (e.g., in color management). However, having been designed for presenting the color of self-luminous objects, these spaces are less suitable for presenting color of reflecting objects. Specifically, they can be used to represent color of objects only for a fixed illumination. Here I put forward a color space to represent the color of objects independently of illumination. It is based on an ideal color atlas comprising the reflectance spectra taking two values: k or 1 - k (0 < or = k < or = 1), with two transitions (at wavelengths lambda(1) and lambda(2)) across the spectrum. This color atlas is complete; that is, every reflecting object is metameric to some element of the atlas. When illumination alters, the classes of metameric reflectance spectra are reshuffled but in each class there is exactly one element of the atlas. Hence, the atlas can uniquely represent the metameric classes irrespective of illumination. Each element of the atlas (thus, object color) is specified by three numbers: (i) lambda = (lambda(1) + lambda(2))/2, which correlates well with hue of object color (as dominant wavelength correlates with hue of light color); (ii) delta =/lambda(1) - lambda/, which correlates with whiteness/blackness; and (iii) alpha =/1 - 2k/, which correlates with chroma of object color (as colorimetric purity correlates with saturation of light color). Using a geographical coordinate system, each element of the atlas (thus, each object color) is geometrically represented as a radius vector so that its length equals alpha, the latitude and longitude being proportional to delta and lambda, respectively.

[1]  Erwin Schrödinger,et al.  Theorie der Pigmente von größter Leuchtkraft , 1920 .

[2]  Physikalische Farbenlehre. Von Wilhelm Ostwald. 2., vermehrte und verbesserte Auflage. Mit 71 Figuren im Text. Der Farbenlehre 2. Buch. Leipzig 1923. Verlag Unesma G. m. b. H., Leipzig , 1924 .

[3]  Nikolaus Nyberg Zum Aufbau des Farbenkörpers im Raume aller Lichtempfindungen , 1929 .

[4]  D. L. Macadam The Theory of the Maximum Visual Efficiency of Colored Materials , 1935 .

[5]  D. B. Judd,et al.  Color in Business, Science, and Industry , 1953 .

[6]  G. Brindley,et al.  The colour of light of very long wavelength , 1955, The Journal of physiology.

[7]  Gunther Wyszecki,et al.  Color Science: Concepts and Methods, Quantitative Data and Formulae, 2nd Edition , 2000 .

[8]  D. Krantz Color Measurement and Color Theory: I. Representation Theorem for Grassmann Structures , 1975 .

[9]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Spectral sensitivity of the foveal cone photopigments between 400 and 500 nm , 1975, Vision Research.

[10]  R. A. Weale,et al.  Color in Business, Science and Industry, 3rd ed , 1976 .

[11]  Anders Hård,et al.  NCS—Natural Color System: A Swedish Standard for Color Notation , 1981 .

[12]  Michael H. Brill,et al.  Conditions under which Schrödinger object colors are optimal , 1983 .

[13]  J. Mollon,et al.  Colour vision : physiology and psychophysics , 1983 .

[14]  Jaap Van Brakel,et al.  Foundations of measurement , 1983 .

[15]  Jan J. Koenderink,et al.  Color atlas theory , 1987 .

[16]  Patrick Suppes,et al.  Geometrical, Threshold, and Probabilistic Representations , 1989 .

[17]  Patrick Suppes,et al.  Foundations of Measurement, Vol. II: Geometrical, Threshold, and Probabilistic Representations , 1989 .

[18]  J. Pokorny,et al.  Full-spectrum cone sensitivity functions for X-chromosome-linked anomalous trichromats. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[19]  M. Bass Fundamentals, techniques, and design , 1995 .

[20]  Vincent Walsh,et al.  Perceptual Constancy: why things look as they do , 1998 .

[21]  Karl R. Gegenfurtner,et al.  Color Vision: From Genes to Perception , 1999 .

[22]  A. Stockman,et al.  The spectral sensitivities of the middle- and long-wavelength-sensitive cones derived from measurements in observers of known genotype , 2000, Vision Research.

[23]  K. Donner,et al.  In search of the visual pigment template , 2000, Visual Neuroscience.

[24]  Gaurav Sharma Digital Color Imaging Handbook , 2002 .

[25]  Laurence T. Maloney Surface Colour Perception and Environmental Constraints , 2003 .

[26]  Donald I. A. MacLeod,et al.  A computational analysis of colour constancy , 2003 .

[27]  Rolf G. Kuehni,et al.  Color Space and Its Divisions , 2003 .

[28]  S. Shevell The Science of Color , 2003 .

[29]  J. Pokorny,et al.  3 – Color Matching and Color Discrimination , 2003 .

[30]  Yoshinobu Nayatani,et al.  Transformation from Munsell Space to Swedish NCS-Through Nayatani Theoretical Space , 2004 .

[31]  Open Problems on the Validity of Grassmann's Laws , 2007 .

[32]  D. L. Macadam The Theory of the Maximum Visual Efficiency of Colored Materials , 1935 .