Effects of the neonatal intensive care unit on auditory attention and distraction.

A theory is proposed that attention to acoustic signals is important for normal development and varies with background masking sounds. Specifically, the theory states that distractibility increases with decreasing predictability of the acoustic environment and with decreasing age. Literature from premature neonates, normal infants, preadolescent children, children with attention deficit disorders, and adults is reviewed. One conclusion is that an environment perceived by adults as predictable may be distracting for preterm infants. One recommendation for future research is to include measures of background acoustic predictability as independent variables or covariates in developmental studies.

[1]  S. S. Stevens,et al.  Critical Band Width in Loudness Summation , 1957 .

[2]  P. Rochat,et al.  Multimodal perception in the control of infant reaching. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  K. Stjernqvist,et al.  Developmental outcome, child behaviour and mother-child interaction at 3 years of age following Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Intervention Program (NIDCAP) intervention. , 2000, Early human development.

[4]  Jack M Fletcher,et al.  Dissociation of sensitivity and response bias in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder during central auditory masking. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[5]  R. Abrams,et al.  Fetal Exposures to Sound and Vibroacoustic Stimulation , 2000, Journal of Perinatology.

[6]  L. Werner Interpreting developmental psychoacoustics. , 1992 .

[7]  P. Jusczyk The discovery of spoken language , 1997 .

[8]  G B Arden,et al.  The Visual System , 2021, AMA Guides to the Evaluation of Permanent Impairment, 6th Edition, 2021.

[9]  A. Decasper,et al.  Of Human Bonding: Newborns Prefer Their Mothers' Voices , 1980 .

[10]  P G Stelmachowicz,et al.  The relation between stimulus context, speech audibility, and perception for normal-hearing and hearing-impaired children. , 2000, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[11]  A Boothroyd,et al.  Speech recognition with reduced spectral cues as a function of age. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[12]  M. Busnel,et al.  Differential fetal auditory reactiveness as a function of stimulus characteristics and state. , 1989, Seminars in perinatology.

[13]  K. Boike,et al.  Infants' sensitivity to broadband noise. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[14]  L. Gray Interactions between sensory and nonsensory factors in the responses of newborn birds to sound. , 1992 .

[15]  H. Lagercrantz,et al.  Indications of improved cognitive development at one year of age among infants born very prematurely who received care based on the Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program (NIDCAP). , 2002, Early human development.

[16]  A. Decasper,et al.  Prenatal experience with low-frequency maternal-voice sounds influence neonatal perception of maternal voice samples , 1987 .

[17]  F. Duffy,et al.  A Three-Center, Randomized, Controlled Trial of Individualized Developmental Care for Very Low Birth Weight Preterm Infants: Medical, Neurodevelopmental, Parenting, and Caregiving Effects , 2003, Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP.

[18]  J. Fletcher,et al.  Continuum of impulsiveness caused by auditory masking. , 2002, International journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology.

[19]  W. Fifer,et al.  Evidence of Transnatal Auditory Learning , 2000, Journal of Perinatology.

[20]  A Wingfield,et al.  One voice too many: adult age differences in language processing with different types of distracting sounds. , 1999, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[21]  R. Lickliter The Role of Sensory Stimulation in Perinatal Development: Insights from Comparative Research for Care of the High‐Risk Infant , 2000, Journal of developmental and behavioral pediatrics : JDBP.

[22]  M. Spence,et al.  Newborn infants prefer the maternal low-pass filtered voice, but not the maternal whispered voice , 1996 .

[23]  M. Daneman,et al.  How young and old adults listen to and remember speech in noise. , 1995, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[24]  L. Werner,et al.  Adults Listen Selectively; Infants Do Not , 1994 .

[25]  T. Brazelton,et al.  Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale , 1973 .

[26]  Otha Linton,et al.  The federal register , 2003 .

[27]  B. Morris,et al.  Physiological Effects of Sound on the Newborn , 2000, Journal of Perinatology.

[28]  H. Levitt Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics. , 1971, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[29]  P. Hepper,et al.  Newborn and fetal response to maternal voice , 1993 .

[30]  D. Querleu,et al.  [Reaction of the newborn infant less than 2 hours after birth to the maternal voice]. , 1984, Journal de gynecologie, obstetrique et biologie de la reproduction.

[31]  Lincoln Gray Developmental changes in chickens' masked thresholds. , 1993, Developmental psychobiology.

[32]  M Kathleen Philbin,et al.  The Influence of Auditory Experience on the Behavior of Preterm Newborns , 2000, Journal of Perinatology.

[33]  Arthur Wingfield,et al.  Distraction by competing speech in young and older adult listeners. , 2002 .

[34]  Bruce A Schneider,et al.  Comparing the effects of aging and background noise on short-term memory performance. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[35]  P. Tun Fast noisy speech: age differences in processing rapid speech with background noise. , 1998, Psychology and aging.

[36]  R. Lickliter,et al.  The development of infant intersensory perception: advantages of a comparative convergent-operations approach. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[37]  K. Evankovich,et al.  Behavioral effects of prematurity. , 1997, Seminars in perinatology.

[38]  D B Hawkins,et al.  Signal-to-noise ratio advantage of binaural hearing aids and directional microphones under different levels of reverberation. , 1984, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[39]  J. Birnholz,et al.  The development of human fetal hearing. , 1983, Science.

[40]  F. Duffy,et al.  Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence for gestational age effects in healthy preterm and fullterm infants studied two weeks after expected due date. , 1990, Child development.

[41]  D M Green,et al.  Maximum-likelihood procedures and the inattentive observer. , 1995, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[42]  D. M. Green,et al.  Stimulus selection in adaptive psychophysical procedures. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[43]  F. Duffy,et al.  Behavioral differences between preterm and full-term newborns as measured with the APIB system scores: I , 1988 .

[44]  Lincoln Gray,et al.  Changing Levels of Quiet in an Intensive Care Nursery , 2002, Journal of Perinatology.

[45]  Y. Igarashi,et al.  Embryonic development of the human organ of Corti: electron microscopic study. , 1980, International journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology.

[46]  A. Decasper,et al.  Human newborns' perception of male voices: preference, discrimination, and reinforcing value. , 1984, Developmental psychobiology.

[47]  K. Gerhardt Characteristics of the fetal sheep sound environment. , 1989, Seminars in perinatology.

[48]  L. L. Elliott Performance of children aged 9 to 17 years on a test of speech intelligibility in noise using sentence material with controlled word predictability. , 1979, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[49]  J. Thompson,et al.  Noise pollution in the operating theatre , 1990, The Lancet.

[50]  L. Gray,et al.  The effect of an intensive care unit sound environment on the development of habituation in healthy avian neonates. , 1994, Developmental psychobiology.

[51]  A. Decasper,et al.  Fetal reactions to recurrent maternal speech , 1994 .

[52]  C. H. Rodeck,et al.  The newborn brain , 2002 .

[53]  H. Als,et al.  The APIB, an assessment of functional competence in preterm and full-term newborns regardless of gestational age at birth: II , 1988 .

[54]  Lincoln Gray PhD,et al.  Measuring Sound in Hospital Nurseries , 2000, Journal of Perinatology.

[55]  James W. Hall,et al.  Recommended Permissible Noise Criteria for Occupied, Newly Constructed or Renovated Hospital Nurseries , 1999, Journal of Perinatology.

[56]  F. Duffy,et al.  Effectiveness of individualized developmental care for low-risk preterm infants: behavioral and electrophysiologic evidence. , 1995, Pediatrics.

[57]  W. S. Brown,et al.  The perception of speech sounds recorded within the uterus of a pregnant sheep. , 1990, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[58]  L A Hayman,et al.  Preliminary report: changes in vital signs of term newborns during MR. , 1996, AJNR. American journal of neuroradiology.

[59]  F. Duffy,et al.  Individualized developmental care for the very low-birth-weight preterm infant. Medical and neurofunctional effects. , 1994, JAMA.

[60]  D B Hawkins,et al.  Speech recognition in noise and reverberation by school-age children. , 1987, Audiology : official organ of the International Society of Audiology.

[61]  Hiram E. Fitzgerald,et al.  Theory and Research in Behavioral Pediatrics , 1991, Springer US.

[62]  SEOG HEE PARK,et al.  Effect of Noise on the Detection of Rib Fractures by Residents , 1994, Investigative radiology.

[63]  Susan Kemper,et al.  The costs of doing two things at once for young and older adults: talking while walking, finger tapping, and ignoring speech or noise. , 2003, Psychology and aging.

[64]  A P Smith,et al.  The combined effects of occupational health hazards: an experimental investigation of the effects of noise, nightwork and meals , 1987, International archives of occupational and environmental health.

[65]  S. Pavlakis Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale, 2nd ed. , 1986, Neurology.

[66]  N. Minich,et al.  Longitudinal outcomes of very low birth weight: Neuropsychological findings , 2004, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[67]  Robert A Lutfi,et al.  Children's detection of pure-tone signals: informational masking with contralateral maskers. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[68]  A. Decasper,et al.  Fetal discrimination of low-pitched musical notes. , 2000, Developmental psychobiology.

[69]  R. Litovsky Developmental changes in the precedence effect: estimates of minimum audible angle. , 1997, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.