Qualitative differences of divalent salts: multidimensional scaling and cluster analysis.

Sensations from salts of iron, calcium, magnesium, and zinc with different anions were studied using a sorting task and multidimensional scaling (MDS). Ten divalent salts were adjusted in concentrations such that the mean intensity ratings were approximately equal. Stimuli were sorted on the basis of similarity to minimize any semantic influence and were examined with and without nasal occlusion to eliminate retronasal cues. Compounds representing the four primary tastes and astringency were also sorted. Similarity estimates were derived from sorting and were submitted to MDS. Divalent salts fell outside the area of the space defined by the four primary tastes. The nose-open condition showed that some of the divalent salts have unique metallic sensations along with astringency. The groupings obtained were corroborated using single-linkage cluster analysis. An iron group was most distinctive in metallic sensations; calcium and magnesium salts were primarily bitter; and zinc salts were characterized by astringency. When nasal cues were not available, the sensations from the divalent salts were mainly explained by bitterness and astringency. Results were consistent with a previous evaluation of divalent salts using descriptive analysis.

[1]  J. C. Smith,et al.  Pre-Bötzinger complex: a brainstem region that may generate respiratory rhythm in mammals. , 1991, Science.

[2]  A. W. MacRae,et al.  Perceptual variation among drinking waters: the reliability of sorting and ranking data for multidimensional scaling , 1997 .

[3]  H. Lawless,et al.  DESCRIPTIVE ANALYSIS OF DIVALENT SALTS. , 2005, Journal of sensory studies.

[4]  K. Kurihara,et al.  Rat taste nerve responses to salts carrying cations of large molecular size; are the taste responses to the salts induced by cation transport across apical membranes of taste cells? , 1991, Comparative biochemistry and physiology. A, Comparative physiology.

[5]  P. Howgate,et al.  Patterns of odour similarity among carbonyls and their mixtures , 1992 .

[6]  R. Keast The Effect of Zinc on Human Taste Perception , 2003 .

[7]  S. Schiffman,et al.  A psychophysical model for gustatory quality. , 1971, Physiology & behavior.

[8]  Harry T. Lawless,et al.  Multidimensional scaling of sorting data applied to cheese perception , 1995 .

[9]  P. Howgate,et al.  Assessing the similarity of odours by sorting and by triadic comparison , 1990 .

[10]  Susan S. Schiffman,et al.  Qualitative differences among sweeteners , 1979, Physiology & Behavior.

[11]  Harry T. Lawless,et al.  CONSISTENCY OF MULTIDIMENSIONAL SCALING MODELS DERIVED FROM ODOR SORTING , 1990 .

[12]  W. E. Myers,et al.  Role of olfaction in perception of non-traditional ‘taste’ stimuli , 1990 .

[13]  M. Tordoff Some basic psychophysics of calcium salt solutions. , 1996, Chemical senses.

[14]  H. Lawless,et al.  Time–intensity characteristics of iron compounds , 2006 .

[15]  H. Lawless,et al.  Oral sensations from iron and copper sulfate , 2005, Physiology & Behavior.

[16]  Morten Meilgaard,et al.  Sensory Evaluation Techniques , 2020 .

[17]  David J. Wild,et al.  MINITAB Release 14 , 2005, J. Chem. Inf. Model..

[18]  H. Lawless Exploration of fragrance categories and ambiguous odors using multidimensional scaling and cluster analysis , 1989 .

[19]  S. Schiffman Taste quality and neural coding Implications from psychophysics and neurophysiology , 2000, Physiology & Behavior.

[20]  J. Kruskal Multidimensional scaling by optimizing goodness of fit to a nonmetric hypothesis , 1964 .

[21]  H. Lawless,et al.  The taste of calcium and magnesium salts and anionic modifications , 2003 .

[22]  J. Hayes,et al.  Capsaicin as a probe of the relationship between bitter taste and chemesthesis , 2003, Physiology & Behavior.

[23]  H. Lawless,et al.  Time-course of astringent sensations , 1991 .

[24]  J. Delwiche,et al.  Are there ‘basic’ tastes? , 1996 .

[25]  Q. Ye,et al.  Voltage dependence of the rat chorda tympani response to Na+ salts: implications for the functional organization of taste receptor cells. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  M. Falahee,et al.  Consumer appraisal of drinking water: Multidimensional scaling analysis , 1995 .

[27]  Q. Ye,et al.  The anion paradox in sodium taste reception: resolution by voltage-clamp studies. , 1991, Science.

[28]  H. Lawless,et al.  Detection thresholds and taste qualities of iron salts , 2006 .

[29]  B. Green,et al.  Oral astringency: a tactile component of flavor. , 1993, Acta psychologica.

[30]  J. Kruskal Nonmetric multidimensional scaling: A numerical method , 1964 .

[31]  D. A. Stevens,et al.  Semantic-free scaling of odor quality , 1996, Physiology & Behavior.

[32]  A. Faurion,et al.  A new specific ageusia: some humans cannot taste L-glutamate. , 2002, Chemical senses.

[33]  M. O'Mahony,et al.  TASTE QUALITY DESCRIPTIONS: CAN THE SUBJECT'S RESPONSE BE AFFECTED BY MENTIONING TASTE WORDS IN THE INSTRUCTIONS? , 1977 .

[34]  K. Chapman,et al.  Metallic taste and retronasal smell. , 2004, Chemical senses.

[35]  D. A. Stevens,et al.  Metallic taste from electrical and chemical stimulation. , 2005, Chemical senses.

[36]  R. Moncrieff,et al.  The chemical senses , 1967 .

[37]  J. H. Thorngate,et al.  Oral sensations associated with the flavan-3-ols (+)-catechin and (-)-epicatechin 1 Journal Series P , 1999 .

[38]  J. Delwiche,et al.  Anion Size of Sodium Salts and Simple Taste Reaction Times , 1999, Physiology & Behavior.

[39]  B. Green,et al.  Psychophysical evidence that oral astringency is a tactile sensation , 1993 .

[40]  H. Lawless,et al.  Astringency of organic acids is related to pH. , 1996, Chemical senses.

[41]  S. Schiffman,et al.  The range of taste quality of sodium salts , 1980, Physiology & Behavior.