Comprehension of Pictorial Symbols: Effects of Context and Test Method

This research examined two factors involved in the evaluation of pictorial symbol comprehension: context (absence vs. presence of photographs depicting the probable environments where a symbol would be seen) and test method (multiple-choice with less vs. more plausible distractor alternatives vs. open-ended). We tested 33 pictorial symbols from various sources. The results showed that the multiple-choice test with less plausible distractors inflated comprehension scores by an average of 30% compared with the other two tests, which did not differ. The presence of context increased symbol comprehension in the open-ended test and in the multiple-choice test that had more plausible distractors. Extensive preliminary procedures demonstrated the difficulty of forming a multiple-choice test with plausible distractor alternatives. This fact, combined with multiple-choice tests′ low ecological validity in reflecting the real-world task of symbol comprehension, suggests that this test should be avoided in favor of an open-ended testing procedure. It is suggested that context provides ecologically valid cues that limit the range of possible constructs that the pictorial symbol could be, raising comprehension scores. The use of context may help reduce the costs (money, time, effort) of producing pictorial symbols with acceptable, above-criterion comprehension levels.

[1]  Belinda Lowenhaupt Collins,et al.  Use of hazard pictorials symbols in the minerals industry , 1983 .

[2]  Leslie A. Whitaker,et al.  The Effects of Context on the Comprehension of Graphic Symbols , 1993 .

[3]  M. Cahill,et al.  Interpretability of graphic symbols as a function of context and experience factors. , 1975 .

[4]  R. Easterby,et al.  Developing effective symbols for public information: The ISO testing procedure : In: Proceedings of the 8th Congress of the International Ergonomics Association, Tokyo, Japan, Ausut 23–27, 1982, Edited by K. Noro, pp 512–513 , 1983 .

[5]  Neil D. Lerner,et al.  The assessment of safety symbol understandability by different testing methods , 1980 .

[6]  United States Pharmacopoeial Convention. , 1910, California state journal of medicine.

[7]  Michael S. Wogalter,et al.  The Effect of Constructing Multiple-Choice Distractor Items around a Single Target Alternative , 1987 .

[8]  Stuart Katz,et al.  Answering Reading Comprehension Items without Passages on the SAT , 1990 .

[9]  G. Lautenschlager,et al.  Answering reading Comprehension Items Without Passages on the SAT-I, the ACT, and the GRE , 1994 .

[10]  E. Tulving,et al.  Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words , 1966 .

[11]  Harm J. G. Zwaga,et al.  Selecting Comprehensible Warning Symbols for Swimming Pool Slides , 1989 .

[12]  I. Biederman,et al.  Searching for objects in real-world scences. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.

[13]  tephen E. Palmer The effects of contextual scenes on the identification of objects , 1975, Memory & cognition.

[14]  H. J. G. Zwaga,et al.  Information design : the design and evaluation of signs and printed material , 1984 .

[15]  M. Wogalter,et al.  Comprehension and retention of safety pictorials , 1997 .

[16]  Paul Green,et al.  THE DEVELOPMENT AND TESTING OF WARNINGS FOR AUTOMOTIVE LIFTS , 1989 .

[17]  M. Wogalter,et al.  Suggestiveness in photospread line‐ups: Similarity induces distinctiveness , 1992 .