Learning to Optimize Speed, Accuracy, and Energy Expenditure: A Framework for Understanding Speed-Accuracy Relations in Goal-Directed Aiming
暂无分享,去创建一个
Luc Tremblay | Digby Elliott | D. Elliott | L. Tremblay | J. Mendoza | Steve Hansen | Jocelyn Mendoza | Steven Hansen
[1] L. Carlton. Processing visual feedback information for movement control. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] W. Helsen,et al. Coupling of Eye, Finger, Elbow, and Shoulder Movements During Manual Aiming , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.
[3] C. I. Howarth,et al. Movement Control in a Repetitive Motor Task , 1970, Nature.
[4] P. Fitts,et al. INFORMATION CAPACITY OF DISCRETE MOTOR RESPONSES. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.
[5] D Elliott,et al. Optimizing the use of Vision in Manual Aiming: The Role of Practice , 1995, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[6] R A Abrams,et al. Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.
[7] P. Matthews. Information processing in motor control and learning George E. Stelmach (ed.). Academic Press, New York (1978) 315 pp. $21.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.
[8] Sascha E Engelbrecht,et al. The Undershoot Bias , 2003, Psychological science.
[9] L. Proteau,et al. Development of Multiple Movement Representations With Practice: Specificity Versus Flexibility , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.
[10] Romeo Chua,et al. Goal-Directed Aiming: Correcting a Force-Specification Error With the Right and Left Hands. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.
[11] D. Meyer,et al. Models for the speed and accuracy of aimed movements. , 1982, Psychological review.
[12] Michael A. Khan,et al. The Effect of Practice on the Control of Rapid Aiming Movements: Evidence for an Interdependency between Programming and Feedback Processing , 1998 .
[13] Steven W. Keele,et al. Movement control in skilled motor performance. , 1968 .
[14] C. Dugas,et al. On the type of information used to control and learn an aiming movement after moderate and extensive training , 1987 .
[15] Matthew Heath,et al. The control of goal-directed limb movements: Correcting errors in the trajectory , 1999 .
[16] K. Newell,et al. Metabolic energy expenditure and the regulation of movement economy , 1998 .
[17] W. Helsen,et al. A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.
[18] Digby Elliott,et al. Rescaling an acquired discrete aiming movement: Specific or general motor learning? , 1997 .
[19] Romeo Chua,et al. Discrete vs. continuous visual control of manual aiming , 1991 .
[20] Y. Guiard. On Fitts's and Hooke's laws: simple harmonic movement in upper-limb cyclical aiming. , 1993, Acta psychologica.
[21] D. Glencross,et al. Response amendments during manual aiming movements to double-step targets. , 1989, Acta psychologica.
[22] J. Brebner. Continuing and reversing the direction of responding movements: some exceptions to the so-called "psychological refractory period". , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.
[23] Les G. Carlton. Processing visual feedback information for movement control. , 1981 .
[24] D. Elliott,et al. Visual regulation of manual aiming , 1993 .
[25] C J Worringham,et al. Variability effects on the internal structure of rapid aiming movements. , 1991, Journal of motor behavior.
[26] R A Abrams,et al. Rapid aimed limb movements: differential effects of practice on component submovements. , 1993, Journal of motor behavior.
[27] H. Zelaznik,et al. Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.
[28] Romeo Chua,et al. On-line control of rapid aiming movements: Unexpected target perturbations and movement kinematics. , 1998 .
[29] H. Zelaznik,et al. Sources of Inaccuracy in Rapid Movement , 1978 .
[30] Michael A. Khan,et al. The Effect of Practice on Component Submovements is Dependent on the Availability of Visual Feedback , 2000, Journal of motor behavior.
[31] L Proteau,et al. Visual Perception Modifies Goal-directed Movement Control: Supporting Evidence from a Visual Perturbation Paradigm , 1997, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[32] W D Beggs,et al. The accuracy of aiming at a target. Some further evidence for a theory of intermittent control. , 1972, Acta psychologica.
[33] Les G. Carlton,et al. Visual Information: The Control of Aiming Movements , 1981 .
[34] P. Fitts. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[35] J. Pratt,et al. Practice and Component Submovements: The Roles of Programming and Feedback in Rapid Aimed Limb Movements. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.
[36] E. R. Crossman,et al. Feedback Control of Hand-Movement and Fitts' Law , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[37] Les G. Carlton,et al. Chapter 1 Visual Processing Time and the Control of Movement , 1992 .
[38] Robert Sessions Woodworth,et al. THE ACCURACY OF VOLUNTARY MOVEMENT , 1899 .
[39] S. A. Wallace,et al. Visual Control of Discrete Aiming Movements , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[40] L G Carlton,et al. The effects of temporal-precision and time-minimization constraints on the spatial and temporal accuracy of aimed hand movements. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.
[41] R. Schmidt. Control Processes In Motor Skills , 1976, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.
[42] L Proteau,et al. A Sensorimotor Basis for Motor Learning: Evidence Indicating Specificity of Practice , 1992, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.