Color-object interference in young children: A Stroop effect in children 3½-6½ years old.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] P. Cramer. The Stroop effect in preschool aged children: a preliminary study. , 1967, The Journal of genetic psychology.
[2] Linda B. Smith,et al. The importance of shape in early lexical learning , 1988 .
[3] J. Booth,et al. Resampling-Based Multiple Testing. , 1994 .
[4] R. Littell. SAS System for Mixed Models , 1996 .
[5] J R Simon,et al. Effects of S-R mapping and response modality on performance in a Stroop Task. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] M. Kramer. Concept formation. , 1970, Nursing Research Conference.
[7] W. J. Langford. Statistical Methods , 1959, Nature.
[8] M. G. Colby,et al. Genetic studies in abstraction. , 1942 .
[9] Wayne D. Gray,et al. Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.
[10] Paul A. Herzberg,et al. B fibers are the best predictors of cardiac activity during Vagus nerve stimulation , 2018, Bioelectronic Medicine.
[11] Gordon A. Hale,et al. Children's attention to stimulus components with variation in relative salience of components and degree of stimulus integration ☆ , 1976 .
[12] U. Mayr,et al. Is there an age deficit in the selection of mental sets? , 2001 .
[13] J. Kagan,et al. Form, color, and size in children's conceptual behavior. , 1961 .
[14] Judith D. Singer,et al. Using SAS PROC MIXED to Fit Multilevel Models, Hierarchical Models, and Individual Growth Models , 1998 .
[15] H. Volkelt,et al. Fortschritte der experimentellen Kinderpsychologie , 1926 .
[16] Colin M. Macleod. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.
[17] J. Driver,et al. Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .
[18] I. Biederman,et al. Surface versus edge-based determinants of visual recognition , 1988, Cognitive Psychology.
[19] Gerhard Strube,et al. The Cognitive Psychology of Knowledge , 1993 .
[20] Translation and competition among internal representations in a reverse Stroop effect , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.
[21] S. Los,et al. Identifying stimuli of different perceptual categories in mixed blocks of trials: evidence for cost in switching between computational processes. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[22] M. Mitler,et al. Dimension preference and performance on a series of concept identification tasks in kindergarten, first-grade, and third-grade children , 1969 .
[23] A P Shimamura,et al. Mapping symbols to response modalities: Interference effects on Stroop-like tasks , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.
[24] P. Bloom,et al. How specific is the shape bias? , 2003, Child development.
[25] J. Davidoff,et al. The colour cognition of children , 1993, Cognition.
[26] Adele Diamond,et al. Not Quite as Grown-Up as We Like to Think , 2005, Psychological science.
[27] R. Melkman,et al. Memory Functioning as Related to Developmental Changes in Bases of Organization. , 1977 .
[28] F. Goodenough,et al. The relative potency of color and form perception at various ages , 1929 .
[29] J. Ridley. Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .
[30] Arthur F. Kramer,et al. fMRI Studies of Stroop Tasks Reveal Unique Roles of Anterior and Posterior Brain Systems in Attentional Selection , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[31] E. Markman,et al. Young children's inductions from natural kinds: the role of categories and appearances. , 1987, Child development.
[32] J. Vauclair,et al. Hand-movement profiles in a tactual—tactual matching task: Effects of spatial factors and laterality , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[33] Daniel Kersten,et al. Is Color an Intrinsic Property of Object Representation? , 2003, Perception.
[34] R. Mclean,et al. A Unified Approach to Mixed Linear Models , 1991 .
[35] S. Los,et al. On the origin of mixing costs: exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials , 1996 .
[36] W. Glaser,et al. Chapter 1 Colors as Properties: Stroop-Like Effects between Objects and their Colors , 1993 .
[37] T. Trabasso,et al. Color and form preference in young children. , 1966, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[38] J. Campione,et al. Color dominance in preschool children as a function of specific cue preferences. , 1971, Child development.
[39] Susan S. Jones,et al. Late talkers show no shape bias in a novel name extension task , 2003 .
[40] Visual and Haptic Dimensional Preference: A Developmental Study. , 1970 .
[41] Linda B. Smith,et al. Object properties and knowledge in early lexical learning. , 1991, Child development.
[42] P. Bloom. How children learn the meanings of words , 2000 .
[43] S. Siegel,et al. Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences , 2022, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.
[44] R. Proctor,et al. The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[45] N. L. Corah. Color and Form in Children's Perceptual Behavior , 1964, Perceptual and motor skills.
[46] Linda B. Smith,et al. Object name Learning Provides On-the-Job Training for Attention , 2002, Psychological science.
[47] M. W. Weir,et al. Role of dimensional dominance in reversal and nonreversal shift behavior , 1966 .
[48] R. Golinkoff,et al. Automatic semantic processing in a picture-word interference task. , 1975 .
[49] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Localisation de l'interférence forme/couleur au niveau perceptuel dans une tâche de type Stroop avec des stimuli-dessins , 1984 .
[50] A. Treisman,et al. The Stroop Test: Selective Attention to Colours and Words , 1969, Nature.
[51] A. Allport,et al. Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs” , 2000, Psychological research.
[52] U. Lindenberger,et al. Adult age differences in task switching. , 2000, Psychology and aging.
[53] S. Rauch,et al. The counting stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging—validation study with functional MRI , 1998, Human brain mapping.