Higher social tolerance is associated with more complex facial behavior in macaques

The social complexity hypothesis for communicative complexity posits that animal societies with more complex social systems require more complex communication systems. We tested the social complexity hypothesis on three macaque species that vary in their degree of social tolerance and complexity. We coded facial behavior in >3000 social interactions across three social contexts (aggressive, submissive, affiliative) in 389 animals, using the Facial Action Coding System for macaques (MaqFACS). We quantified communicative complexity using three measures of uncertainty: entropy, specificity, and prediction error. We found that the relative entropy of facial behavior was higher for the more tolerant crested macaques as compared to the less tolerant Barbary and rhesus macaques across all social contexts, indicating that crested macaques more frequently use a higher diversity of facial behavior. The context specificity of facial behavior was higher in rhesus as compared to Barbary and crested macaques, demonstrating that Barbary and crested macaques used facial behavior more flexibly across different social contexts. Finally, a random forest classifier predicted social context from facial behavior with highest accuracy for rhesus and lowest for crested, indicating there is higher uncertainty and complexity in the facial behavior of crested macaques. Overall, our results support the social complexity hypothesis.

[1]  C. Boeckx,et al.  What is simple is actually quite complex: A critical note on terminology in the domain of language and communication. , 2022, Journal of comparative psychology.

[2]  P. Kappeler,et al.  Coevolution of social and communicative complexity in lemurs , 2022, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[3]  G. Schino,et al.  Variation in communicative complexity in relation to social structure and organization in non-human primates , 2022, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[4]  K. Liebal,et al.  The social dynamics of complex gestural communication in great and lesser apes (Pan troglodytes, Pongo abelii, Symphalangus syndactylus) , 2022, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[5]  Peter R. Clark,et al.  Crested macaque facial movements are more intense and stereotyped in potentially risky social interactions , 2022, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[6]  B. Thierry,et al.  Tolerant and intolerant macaques differ in the context specificity of their calls and how they ‘comment’ on the interactions of others , 2022, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[7]  K. Slocombe,et al.  The language void 10 years on: multimodal primate communication research is still uncommon , 2022, Ethology Ecology & Evolution.

[8]  B. Thierry Where do we stand with the covariation framework in primate societies? , 2021, American journal of biological anthropology.

[9]  B. Thierry,et al.  Measuring complexity in organisms and organizations , 2021, Royal Society Open Science.

[10]  Alexander Mielke,et al.  NetFACS: Using network science to understand facial communication systems , 2020, Behavior Research Methods.

[11]  Peter R. Clark,et al.  Morphological variants of silent bared-teeth displays have different social interaction outcomes in crested macaques (Macaca nigra). , 2020, American journal of physical anthropology.

[12]  B. Thierry,et al.  Tolerant and intolerant macaques show different levels of structural complexity in their vocal communication , 2020, Proceedings of the Royal Society B.

[13]  C. M. Berman,et al.  Avoidant social style among wild crested macaque males (Macaca nigra) in Tangkoko Nature Reserve, Sulawesi, Indonesia , 2020, Behaviour.

[14]  H. Ogawa,et al.  Comparative ecological and behavioral study of Macaca assamensis and M. mulatta in Shivapuri Nagarjun National Park, Nepal , 2020, Primates.

[15]  B. Waller,et al.  Measuring the evolution of facial ‘expression’ using multi-species FACS , 2020, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[16]  Aleix M. Martinez,et al.  Emotional Expressions Reconsidered: Challenges to Inferring Emotion From Human Facial Movements , 2019, Psychological science in the public interest : a journal of the American Psychological Society.

[17]  G. Schino,et al.  Social complexity from within: how individuals experience the structure and organization of their groups , 2019, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[18]  P. Kappeler,et al.  Clarifying and expanding the social complexity hypothesis for communicative complexity , 2019, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[19]  P. Kappeler A framework for studying social complexity , 2019, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[20]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  Flexible usage and social function in primate vocalizations , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  B. Waller,et al.  Rethinking primate facial expression: A predictive framework , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[22]  J. Fischer,et al.  Meaning, intention, and inference in primate vocal communication , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[23]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  The origin of meaning in animal signals , 2017, Animal Behaviour.

[24]  Marco Gamba,et al.  BORIS: a free, versatile open‐source event‐logging software for video/audio coding and live observations , 2016 .

[25]  Peter Schuster,et al.  How complexity originates: Examples from history reveal additional roots to complexity , 2016, Complex..

[26]  B. Waller,et al.  Macaques can predict social outcomes from facial expressions , 2016, Animal Cognition.

[27]  B. Waller,et al.  MaqFACS (Macaque Facial Action Coding System) can be used to document facial movements in Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) , 2015, PeerJ.

[28]  Andreas Ziegler,et al.  ranger: A Fast Implementation of Random Forests for High Dimensional Data in C++ and R , 2015, 1508.04409.

[29]  J. Beehner,et al.  Measuring social complexity , 2015, Animal Behaviour.

[30]  Asif A. Ghazanfar,et al.  The evolution of speech: vision, rhythm, cooperation , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  A. Ghazanfar,et al.  The neurobiology of primate vocal communication , 2014, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[32]  R. Diogo,et al.  Plain faces are more expressive: comparative study of facial colour, mobility and musculature in primates , 2014, Biology Letters.

[33]  Asif A Ghazanfar,et al.  Dynamic faces speed up the onset of auditory cortical spiking responses during vocal detection , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Graded or discrete? A quantitative analysis of Campbell's monkey alarm calls , 2013, Animal Behaviour.

[35]  A. Chait,et al.  Comparison of the terrestriality of Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) between tourist and wild sites in the region of Ouzoud, Morocco , 2013 .

[36]  B. Thierry,et al.  Social Tolerance in Wild Female Crested Macaques (Macaca nigra) in Tangkoko-Batuangus Nature Reserve, Sulawesi, Indonesia , 2013, American journal of primatology.

[37]  Bernard Thierry,et al.  Hierarchical Steepness, Counter‐Aggression, and Macaque Social Style Scale , 2012, American journal of primatology.

[38]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[39]  S. Dobson Coevolution of Facial Expression and Social Tolerance in Macaques , 2012, American journal of primatology.

[40]  C. Sueur,et al.  A comparative network analysis of social style in macaques , 2011, Animal Behaviour.

[41]  L. Parr,et al.  Brief communication: MaqFACS: A muscle-based facial movement coding system for the rhesus macaque. , 2010, American journal of physical anthropology.

[42]  Kate E. Jones,et al.  PanTHERIA: a species‐level database of life history, ecology, and geography of extant and recently extinct mammals , 2009 .

[43]  S. Dobson Socioecological correlates of facial mobility in nonhuman anthropoids. , 2009, American journal of physical anthropology.

[44]  Odile Petit,et al.  A comparative study of conflict resolution in macaques: insights into the nature of trait covariation , 2008, Animal Behaviour.

[45]  B. Thierry,et al.  Unity in diversity: Lessons from macaque societies , 2007 .

[46]  Evandro Agazzi,et al.  What is Complexity , 2002 .

[47]  M. Kinnaird,et al.  Behavior, Diet, and Movements of the Sulawesi Crested Black Macaque (Macaca nigra) , 1997, International Journal of Primatology.

[48]  R. J. Smith,et al.  Body mass in comparative primatology. , 1997, Journal of human evolution.

[49]  A. Whiten,et al.  On the Nature of Complexity in Cognitive and Behavioural Science , 1997 .

[50]  A. J. Fridlund Human Facial Expression: An Evolutionary View , 1994 .

[51]  Jacob Cohen A Coefficient of Agreement for Nominal Scales , 1960 .

[52]  Katherine B. Martin,et al.  Facial Action Coding System , 2015 .

[53]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[54]  B. Thierry,et al.  Variation in kin bias: species differences and time constraints in macaques , 2010 .

[55]  S. Dobson Allometry of facial mobility in anthropoid primates: implications for the evolution of facial expression. , 2009, American journal of physical anthropology.

[56]  Sang Joon Kim,et al.  A Mathematical Theory of Communication , 2006 .

[57]  Reuben R. McDaniel,et al.  Uncertainty and Surprise in Complex Systems , 2005 .

[58]  Nitesh V. Chawla,et al.  SMOTE: Synthetic Minority Over-sampling Technique , 2002, J. Artif. Intell. Res..

[59]  F. D. de Waal,et al.  The formal hierarchy of rhesus macaques: An investigation of the bared‐teeth display , 1985, American journal of primatology.

[60]  J. Hayashi [Sampling methods]. , 1982, Josanpu zasshi = The Japanese journal for midwife.

[61]  J. Altmann,et al.  Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. , 1974, Behaviour.