Distraction and task engagement: How interesting and boring information impact driving performance and subjective and physiological responses.

As more devices and services are integrated into vehicles, drivers face new opportunities to perform additional tasks while driving. While many studies have explored the detrimental effects of varying task demands on driving performance, there has been little attention devoted to tasks that vary in terms of personal interest or investment-a quality we liken to the concept of task engagement. The purpose of this study was to explore the impact of task engagement on driving performance, subjective appraisals of performance and workload, and various physiological measurements. In this study, 31 participants (M = 37 yrs) completed three driving conditions in a driving simulator: listening to boring auditory material; listening to interesting material; and driving with no auditory material. Drivers were simultaneously monitored using near-infrared spectroscopy, heart monitoring and eye tracking systems. Drivers exhibited less variability in lane keeping and headway maintenance for both auditory conditions; however, response times to critical braking events were longer in the interesting audio condition. Drivers also perceived the interesting material to be less demanding and less complex, although the material was objectively matched for difficulty. Drivers showed a reduced concentration of cerebral oxygenated hemoglobin when listening to interesting material, compared to baseline and boring conditions, yet they exhibited superior recognition for this material. The practical implications, from a safety standpoint, are discussed.

[1]  Klaus Bengler,et al.  “Take over!” How long does it take to get the driver back into the loop? , 2013 .

[2]  Y. Bhambhani,et al.  Cerebral oxygenation and blood volume responses to seated whole-body vibration , 2005, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[3]  H. Mayberg Brain Activation , 1994, Neurology.

[4]  Toshinori Kato,et al.  Functional brain imaging using near-infrared spectroscopy during actual driving on an expressway , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[5]  Hirofumi Saito,et al.  Distinctive activation patterns under intrinsically versus extrinsically driven cognitive loads in prefrontal cortex: A near-infrared spectroscopy study using a driving video game , 2012, Neuroscience Letters.

[6]  M. A. Recarte,et al.  Mental Workload and Visual Impairment: Differences between Pupil, Blink, and Subjective Rating , 2008, The Spanish Journal of Psychology.

[7]  P. Pintrich,et al.  Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. , 1990 .

[8]  D. H. Lee,et al.  Multivariate analysis of mental and physical load components in sinus arrhythmia scores. , 1990, Ergonomics.

[9]  F. Craik,et al.  Levels of Pro-cessing: A Framework for Memory Research , 1975 .

[10]  Albert Kircher,et al.  Using mobile telephones: cognitive workload and attention resource allocation. , 2004, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[11]  Elaine Toms,et al.  What is user engagement? A conceptual framework for defining user engagement with technology , 2008, J. Assoc. Inf. Sci. Technol..

[12]  J. Beatty Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. , 1982 .

[13]  Paul B. Fitzgerald,et al.  A transcranial magnetic stimulation study of the effect of visual orientation on the putative human mirror neuron system , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  Maarten A. S. Boksem,et al.  Absorbed in the task: Personality measures predict engagement during task performance as tracked by error negativity and asymmetrical frontal activity , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[15]  Khalil Sima'an,et al.  Wired for Speech: How Voice Activates and Advances the Human-Computer Relationship , 2006, Computational Linguistics.

[16]  Neil Lerner,et al.  On-Road Study of Willingness to Engage in Distracting Tasks , 2005 .

[17]  Hirohiko Kaneko,et al.  Relationship between Emotional State and Pupil Diameter Variability under Various Types of Workload Stress , 2009, HCI.

[18]  K A Brookhuis,et al.  The effects of mobile telephoning on driving performance. , 1991, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[19]  S. Hart,et al.  Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of Empirical and Theoretical Research , 1988 .

[20]  Gérard Dray,et al.  NIRS-measured prefrontal cortex activity in neuroergonomics: strengths and weaknesses , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[21]  W J Horrey,et al.  Dissociation between driving performance and drivers' subjective estimates of performance and workload in dual-task conditions. , 2009, Journal of safety research.

[22]  Bryan Reimer,et al.  Sensitivity of Physiological Measures for Detecting Systematic Variations in Cognitive Demand From a Working Memory Task , 2012, Hum. Factors.

[23]  Eiko Hatakeyama,et al.  A comparison of cerebral activity in the prefrontal region between young adults and the elderly while driving. , 2007, Journal of physiological anthropology.

[24]  Suzanne Hidi,et al.  Interest and Its Contribution as a Mental Resource for Learning , 1990 .

[25]  J. G. Hollands,et al.  Engineering Psychology and Human Performance , 1984 .

[26]  Nicole Fruehauf Flow The Psychology Of Optimal Experience , 2016 .

[27]  Yan Wang,et al.  Assessment of cerebral oxygenation during prolonged simulated driving using near infrared spectroscopy: its implications for fatigue development , 2009, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[28]  Ralph E. Reynolds,et al.  Effect of Interest on Attention and Learning , 1988 .

[29]  Louis Tijerina,et al.  Driver Workload Metrics Task 2 Final Report , 2006 .

[30]  J. Leon-Carrion,et al.  The hemodynamics of cognitive control: The level of concentration of oxygenated hemoglobin in the superior prefrontal cortex varies as a function of performance in a modified Stroop task , 2008, Behavioural Brain Research.

[31]  F. Thomas Eggemeier,et al.  Workload assessment methodology. , 1986 .

[32]  Natasha Merat,et al.  Transition to manual: driver behaviour when resuming control from a highly automated vehicle , 2014 .

[33]  M. Csíkszentmihályi Flow. The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York (HarperPerennial) 1990. , 1990 .

[34]  J G Hollands,et al.  ENGINEERING PSYCHOLOGY AND HUMAN PERFORMANCE - THIRD EDITION , 2000 .

[35]  M. Tamura,et al.  Detection of dynamic changes in cerebral oxygenation coupled to neuronal function during mental work in man , 1993, Neuroscience Letters.

[36]  Valdimar Briem,et al.  Behavioural effects of mobile telephone use during simulated driving. , 1995 .

[37]  Andrew L. Kun,et al.  On the feasibility of using pupil diameter to estimate cognitive load changes for in-vehicle spoken dialogues , 2013, INTERSPEECH.

[38]  A. Villringer,et al.  Near infrared spectroscopy (NIRS): A new tool to study hemodynamic changes during activation of brain function in human adults , 1993, Neuroscience Letters.

[39]  Chris S Dula,et al.  Differing types of cellular phone conversations and dangerous driving. , 2011, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[40]  John D. Lee,et al.  Driver Distraction : Theory, Effects, and Mitigation , 2008 .

[41]  A. Kramer,et al.  Physiological metrics of mental workload: A review of recent progress , 1990, Multiple-task performance.

[42]  M. Fenske,et al.  The Unengaged Mind , 2012, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[43]  Elaine Chapman,et al.  Alternative Approaches to Assessing Student Engagement Rates , 2003 .

[44]  Marvin J. Dainoff,et al.  Ergonomics and Health Aspects of Work with Computers , 2007, Lecture Notes in Computer Science.