Pip and pop: nonspatial auditory signals improve spatial visual search.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Jan Theeuwes | Christian N L Olivers | Erik Van der Burg | Adelbert W Bronkhorst | E. Van der Burg | J. Theeuwes | A. Bronkhorst | C. Olivers
[1] de Gelder. Sound Enhances Visual Perception: Cross-Modal Effects of Auditory Organization on Vision , 2001 .
[2] J. Theeuwes. Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[3] James T. Enns,et al. Unique Temporal Change Is the Key to Attentional Capture , 2005, Psychological science.
[4] John J. Foxe,et al. Multisensory Convergence in Early Cortical Processing. , 2004 .
[5] John J. Foxe,et al. Multisensory auditory-visual interactions during early sensory processing in humans: a high-density electrical mapping study. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[6] S. Los,et al. Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] K. Saberi,et al. Auditory psychomotor coordination and visual search performance , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[8] Waka Fujisaki,et al. Temporal frequency characteristics of synchrony–asynchrony discrimination of audio-visual signals , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[9] J. Wolfe,et al. Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[10] M. Giard,et al. Auditory-Visual Integration during Multimodal Object Recognition in Humans: A Behavioral and Electrophysiological Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[11] T SHIPLEY,et al. Auditory Flutter-Driving of Visual Flicker , 1964, Science.
[12] R. Näätänen,et al. Foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1981 .
[13] Giovanni Galfano,et al. Nonspatial attentional shifts between audition and vision. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] M. Woldorff,et al. Selective attention and audiovisual integration: is attending to both modalities a prerequisite for early integration? , 2006, Cerebral cortex.
[15] Stephen J. Boies,et al. Components of attention. , 1971 .
[16] R. Campbell,et al. Audiovisual Integration of Speech Falters under High Attention Demands , 2005, Current Biology.
[17] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[18] A. Treisman,et al. Conjunction search revisited. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] J. Theeuwes. Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.
[20] M. Wallace,et al. Representation and integration of multiple sensory inputs in primate superior colliculus. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[21] C. Spence,et al. The Handbook of Multisensory Processing , 2004 .
[22] T Z Strybel,et al. Aurally Aided Visual Search in the Central Visual Field: Effects of Visual Load and Visual Enhancement of the Target , 1991, Human factors.
[23] S. Hackley,et al. Which stages of processing are speeded by a warning signal? , 2003, Biological Psychology.
[24] P Bertelson,et al. The Time Course of Preparation* , 1967, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[25] John J. Foxe,et al. Multisensory contributions to low-level, ‘unisensory’ processing , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[26] Jörg Lewald,et al. Spatio-temporal constraints for auditory–visual integration , 2001, Behavioural Brain Research.
[27] S. Hillyard,et al. Involuntary orienting to sound improves visual perception , 2000, Nature.
[28] Sander A. Los,et al. The effective time course of preparation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.
[29] Piotr Jaśkowski,et al. Temporal-order judgments and reaction time for stimuli of different modalities , 1990, Psychological research.
[30] C. Spence,et al. Attentional capture in serial audiovisual search tasks , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.
[31] Jan Theeuwes,et al. SEARCH FOR A CONJUNCTIVELY DEFINED TARGET CAN BE SELECTIVELY LIMITED TO A COLOR-DEFINED SUBSET OF ELEMENTS , 1995 .
[32] J. Driver,et al. Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[33] C. Bundesen,et al. A neural theory of visual attention: bridging cognition and neurophysiology. , 2005, Psychological review.
[34] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[35] H. Kennedy,et al. Anatomical Evidence of Multimodal Integration in Primate Striate Cortex , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[36] D. H. Warren,et al. Immediate perceptual response to intersensory discrepancy. , 1980, Psychological bulletin.
[37] Robert S. Bolia,et al. Aurally Aided Visual Search in Three-Dimensional Space , 1999, Hum. Factors.
[38] Andrew B. Leber,et al. It’s under control: Top-down search strategies can override attentional capture , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[39] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. Visual search for a target changing in synchrony with an auditory signal , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[40] Daniel Senkowski,et al. Good times for multisensory integration: Effects of the precision of temporal synchrony as revealed by gamma-band oscillations , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[41] J. Lewald,et al. Cross-modal perceptual integration of spatially and temporally disparate auditory and visual stimuli. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[42] A. Treisman,et al. A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.
[43] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[44] M. Masson,et al. Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.