Dorsolateral prefrontal lesions do not impair tests of scene learning and decision-making that require frontal–temporal interaction

Theories of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) involvement in cognitive function variously emphasize its involvement in rule implementation, cognitive control, or working and/or spatial memory. These theories predict broad effects of DLPFC lesions on tests of visual learning and memory. We evaluated the effects of DLPFC lesions (including both banks of the principal sulcus) in rhesus monkeys on tests of scene learning and strategy implementation that are severely impaired following crossed unilateral lesions of frontal cortex and inferotemporal cortex. Dorsolateral lesions had no effect on learning of new scene problems postoperatively, or on the implementation of preoperatively acquired strategies. They were also without effect on the ability to adjust choice behaviour in response to a change in reinforcer value, a capacity that requires interaction between the amygdala and frontal lobe. These intact abilities following DLPFC damage support specialization of function within the prefrontal cortex, and suggest that many aspects of memory and strategic and goal‐directed behaviour can survive ablation of this structure.

[1]  K. Kawamura,et al.  Corticocortical projections to the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey investigated with horseradish peroxidase techniques , 1984, Neuroscience Research.

[2]  J. Duncan,et al.  Encoding Strategies Dissociate Prefrontal Activity from Working Memory Demand , 2003, Neuron.

[3]  H. Seo,et al.  Mechanisms of Reinforcement Learning and Decision Making in the Primate Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[4]  C. Summerfield,et al.  An information theoretical approach to prefrontal executive function , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Prefrontal activity associated with working memory and episodic long-term memory , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  M Petrides,et al.  Impairments on nonspatial self-ordered and externally ordered working memory tasks after lesions of the mid-dorsal part of the lateral frontal cortex in the monkey , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[7]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Connections of inferior temporal areas TEO and TE with parietal and frontal cortex in macaque monkeys. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[8]  D. Gaffan,et al.  Asymmetry of attentional set in rhesus monkeys learning colour and shape discriminations , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[9]  M. Petrides Dissociable Roles of Mid-Dorsolateral Prefrontal and Anterior Inferotemporal Cortex in Visual Working Memory , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  Philip G. F. Browning,et al.  Perseverative interference with object-in-place scene learning in rhesus monkeys with bilateral ablation of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. , 2008, Learning & memory.

[11]  E. Murray,et al.  Bilateral Orbital Prefrontal Cortex Lesions in Rhesus Monkeys Disrupt Choices Guided by Both Reward Value and Reward Contingency , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  E. Murray,et al.  Control of Response Selection by Reinforcer Value Requires Interaction of Amygdala and Orbital Prefrontal Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Mortimer Mishkin,et al.  Visual recognition impairment follows ventromedial but not dorsolateral prefrontal lesions in monkeys , 1986, Behavioural Brain Research.

[14]  D. Gaffan Scene-Specific Memory for Objects: A Model of Episodic Memory Impairment in Monkeys with Fornix Transection , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  P S Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Association of Storage and Processing Functions in the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex of the Nonhuman Primate , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  T. Robbins Dissociating executive functions of the prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[17]  Anna S. Mitchell,et al.  Neurotoxic lesions of ventrolateral prefrontal cortex impair object-in-place scene memory , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[18]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Involvement of human left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in perceptual decision making is independent of response modality , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Effect of Expected Reward Magnitude on the Response of Neurons in the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex of the Macaque , 1999, Neuron.

[20]  J. Fuster,et al.  Prefrontal Cortex and the Bridging of Temporal Gaps in the Perception‐Action Cycle , 1990, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[21]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Functional development of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: An analysis utilizing reversible cryogenic depression , 1978, Brain Research.

[22]  E. Miller,et al.  Neuronal activity in primate dorsolateral and orbital prefrontal cortex during performance of a reward preference task , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[23]  D. Gaffan,et al.  Interaction of Inferior Temporal Cortex with Frontal Cortex and Basal Forebrain: Double Dissociation in Strategy Implementation and Associative Learning , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  S. Wise,et al.  Rule-dependent neuronal activity in the prefrontal cortex , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  David Gaffan,et al.  Against memory systems. , 2002, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[26]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Analysis of the delayed-alternation deficit produced by dorsolateral prefrontal lesions in the rhesus monkey. , 1971, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[27]  H. E. Rosvold,et al.  Localization of function within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the rhesus monkey. , 1970, Experimental neurology.

[28]  Anna S. Mitchell,et al.  Orbital Prefrontal Cortex Is Required for Object-in-Place Scene Memory But Not Performance of a Strategy Implementation Task , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  E. Murray,et al.  Excitotoxic Lesions of the Amygdala Fail to Produce Impairment in Visual Learning for Auditory Secondary Reinforcement But Interfere with Reinforcer Devaluation Effects in Rhesus Monkeys , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  H. Barbas,et al.  Projections from the amygdala to basoventral and mediodorsal prefrontal regions in the rhesus monkey , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[31]  James B. Rowe,et al.  Working Memory for Location and Time: Activity in Prefrontal Area 46 Relates to Selection Rather than Maintenance in Memory , 2001, NeuroImage.

[32]  J M Fuster,et al.  The prefrontal cortex and its relation to behavior. , 1991, Progress in brain research.

[33]  David Badre,et al.  Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Evidence for a Hierarchical Organization of the Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  D. Pandya,et al.  Comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis of the human and the macaque ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and corticocortical connection patterns in the monkey , 2002, The European journal of neuroscience.

[35]  David Gaffan,et al.  The role of prefrontal cortex in object‐in‐place learning in monkeys , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[36]  P. Goldman-Rakic The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[37]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Prefrontal Neural Correlates of Memory for Sequences , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  D. Schacter,et al.  Prefrontal Contributions to Executive Control: fMRI Evidence for Functional Distinctions within Lateral Prefrontal Cortex , 2001, NeuroImage.

[39]  M. Petrides,et al.  Differential activation of the human orbital, mid-ventrolateral, and mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during the processing of visual stimuli , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  D. Pandya,et al.  Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and the macaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[41]  J. Price,et al.  Limbic connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[42]  R. Passingham The frontal lobes and voluntary action , 1993 .

[43]  H. Seo,et al.  Dynamic signals related to choices and outcomes in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  C. Ranganath,et al.  The Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Contributes to Successful Relational Memory Encoding , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  A. Owen,et al.  Fractionating attentional control using event-related fMRI. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[46]  Shintaro Funahashi,et al.  Reward-period Activity in Primate Dorsolateral Prefrontal and Orbitofrontal Neurons Is Affected by Reward Schedules , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  Jun Tanji,et al.  Area-selective neuronal activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex for information retrieval and action planning. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.