The Neural Basis of Difficulties Disengaging From Negative Irrelevant Material in Major Depression

Recurrent uncontrollable negative thoughts are a hallmark of depressive episodes. Deficits in cognitive control have been proposed to underlie this debilitating aspect of depression. Here, we used functional neuroimaging during an emotional working memory (WM) task to elucidate the neural correlates of these difficulties in cognitive control. In a WM manipulation involving depressed participants, the dorsal anterior cingulate and parietal and bilateral insular cortices were activated significantly more when negative words were removed from WM than when they were maintained in WM; in contrast, nondepressed participants exhibited stronger neural activations in these regions for positive than for negative material. These findings implicate anomalous activation of components of the task-positive network, known to be modulated by cognitive effort, in depression-associated difficulties in expelling negative material from WM. Future studies should examine the association between these aberrations and the maintenance of depressive symptoms.

[1]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex , 1999, Nature.

[2]  M. Mintun,et al.  The default mode network and self-referential processes in depression , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[3]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[4]  G. Pearlson,et al.  Abnormal prefrontal activity subserving attentional control of emotion in remitted depressed patients during a working memory task with emotional distracters , 2011, Psychological Medicine.

[5]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  Jutta Joormann,et al.  Attentional bias in dysphoria: The role of inhibitory processes , 2004 .

[7]  T. Paus,et al.  Regional differences in the effects of task difficulty and motor output on blood flow response in the human anterior cingulate cortex: a review of 107 PET activation studies , 1998, Neuroreport.

[8]  A. von Leupoldt,et al.  Specificity of cognitive biases in patients with current depression and remitted depression and in patients with asthma , 2009, Psychological Medicine.

[9]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Neural correlates of inhibitory deficits in depression , 2010, Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging.

[10]  M. Hamilton A RATING SCALE FOR DEPRESSION , 1960, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[11]  Sara M. Levens,et al.  Updating positive and negative stimuli in working memory in depression. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[12]  K. Oberauer Removing irrelevant information from working memory: a cognitive aging study with the modified Sternberg task. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  A. Isen,et al.  Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. , 1984 .

[14]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Working Memory, Comprehension, and Aging: A Review and a New View , 1988 .

[15]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory , 1995, Nature.

[16]  E. Foa,et al.  The effect of practice on recall of emotional information in individuals with generalized social phobia. , 2001, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[17]  Matthew Garner,et al.  Biases in visual orienting to negative and positive scenes in dysphoria: An eye movement study. , 2007, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[18]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Attentional biases for negative interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. , 2004, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[19]  R. N. Spreng,et al.  Reliable differences in brain activity between young and old adults: A quantitative meta-analysis across multiple cognitive domains , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[20]  M. Siemer Mood-congruent cognitions constitute mood experience. , 2005, Emotion.

[21]  Matthew Brett,et al.  Dissociable contributions of the mid-ventrolateral frontal cortex and the medial temporal lobe system to human memory , 2006, NeuroImage.

[22]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  A Three-Dimensional Statistical Analysis for CBF Activation Studies in Human Brain , 1992, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[23]  Christian E. Waugh,et al.  Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci (2011) 11:85–96 , 2010 .

[24]  R. de Raedt,et al.  Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression. , 2006, Journal of affective disorders.

[25]  R. de Raedt,et al.  Rumination mediates the relationship between impaired cognitive control for emotional information and depressive symptoms: A prospective study in remitted depressed adults. , 2012, Behaviour research and therapy.

[26]  D. Pizzagalli Frontocingulate Dysfunction in Depression: Toward Biomarkers of Treatment Response , 2011, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[27]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of working memory: , 2003, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[28]  Mark W. Woolrich,et al.  Constrained linear basis sets for HRF modelling using Variational Bayes , 2004, NeuroImage.

[29]  S. Nolen-Hoeksema,et al.  A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: the 1989 Loma Prieta Earthquake. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[30]  Nicholas D. Walsh,et al.  Neural basis of the emotional Stroop interference effect in major depression , 2007, Psychological Medicine.

[31]  Ian H. Gotlib,et al.  Emotion regulation in depression: Relation to cognitive inhibition , 2010, Cognition & emotion.

[32]  M. Bradley,et al.  Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings , 1999 .

[33]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  R. Dolan,et al.  The neural basis of mood-congruent processing biases in depression. , 2002, Archives of general psychiatry.

[35]  Ramona O Hopkins,et al.  Functional neuroimaging evidence for high cognitive effort on the Word Memory Test in the absence of external incentives , 2007, Brain injury.

[36]  I. Gotlib,et al.  Brooding and Reflection Reconsidered: A Factor Analytic Examination of Rumination in Currently Depressed, Formerly Depressed, and Never Depressed Individuals , 2011, Cognitive Therapy and Research.

[37]  G. Glover,et al.  Spiral‐in/out BOLD fMRI for increased SNR and reduced susceptibility artifacts , 2001, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[38]  Jutta Joormann,et al.  Updating the contents of working memory in depression: interference from irrelevant negative material. , 2008, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[39]  Tor D. Wager,et al.  Common and unique components of response inhibition revealed by fMRI , 2005, NeuroImage.

[40]  A. Beck,et al.  Beck Depression Inventory–II , 2011 .

[41]  S. Lyubomirsky,et al.  Rethinking Rumination , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[42]  Marc G. Berman,et al.  Interference resolution in major depression , 2010, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.