On How to Be Unpredictable

The voluntary task-switching paradigm requires subjects to select randomly between tasks and promises to provide a window into executive task selection independent of exogenous influences present in standard task-switching situations. We show here that the degree to which subjects perseverate on tasks across trials captures unique individual differences variance, but also that the switch rate is under strong stimulus-driven control: “Voluntary” switches are much more frequent when the stimulus changes than when it repeats. Most important, we show that individuals whose no-switch trials are selectively slowed exhibit less perseveration and stimulus-driven effects (and thus more voluntary selection) than individuals whose no-switch trials do not show this slowing. We suggest that selective slowing indicates a strategy of treating trials as discrete events—possibly through inhibition of the preceding task set. These results not only demonstrate massive nonvoluntary influences on voluntary selection that are largely untapped by standard task-switching measures, but also show how such influences can be counteracted through strategic adaptations.

[1]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  Adult age differences in task switching. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[2]  P. Glimcher decisions, uncertainty and the brain , 2003 .

[3]  Jörn Diedrichsen,et al.  Dissociating Task-set Selection from Task-set Inhibition in the Prefrontal Cortex , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  U. Mayr,et al.  Sticky rules: integration between abstract rules and specific actions. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[5]  U. Mayr Age differences in the selection of mental sets: the role of inhibition, stimulus ambiguity, and response-set overlap. , 2001, Psychology and aging.

[6]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  Task inhibition and task repetition in task switching , 2006 .

[7]  N. Meiran Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. , 1996 .

[8]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .

[9]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  S. Keele,et al.  Changing internal constraints on action: the role of backward inhibition. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  B. Hommel Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  A. Rapoport,et al.  Generation of random series in two-person strictly competitive games , 1992 .

[13]  N. Meiran Modeling cognitive control in task-switching , 2000, Psychological research.

[14]  G. Logan,et al.  Clever homunculus: is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  Catherine M Arrington,et al.  The Cost of a Voluntary Task Switch , 2004, Psychological science.

[16]  Catherine M Arrington,et al.  Voluntary task switching: chasing the elusive homunculus. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[17]  S. Monsell,et al.  Costs of a predictible switch between simple cognitive tasks. , 1995 .

[18]  A. Rapoport,et al.  Randomization in individual choice behavior. , 1997 .

[19]  A D Baddeley,et al.  The Capacity for Generating Information by Randomization , 1966, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.