Incomplete information about the partner affects the development of collaborative strategies in joint action
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[2] R C Miall,et al. System Identification Applied to a Visuomotor Task: Near-Optimal Human Performance in a Noisy Changing Task , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[3] M. Ernst,et al. The statistical determinants of adaptation rate in human reaching. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[4] S. Vajda,et al. GAMES AND DECISIONS; INTRODUCTION AND CRITICAL SURVEY. , 1958 .
[5] M. Cripps. The theory of learning in games. , 1999 .
[6] Mitsuo Kawato,et al. Physically interacting individuals estimate the partner’s goal to enhance their movements , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.
[7] T. Başar,et al. Dynamic Noncooperative Game Theory, 2nd Edition , 1998 .
[8] K. Shapiro,et al. The contingent negative variation (CNV) event-related potential (ERP) predicts the attentional blink , 2008 .
[9] O. H. Brownlee,et al. ACTIVITY ANALYSIS OF PRODUCTION AND ALLOCATION , 1952 .
[10] U. Alon,et al. The mirror game as a paradigm for studying the dynamics of two people improvising motion together , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[11] Junya Masumoto,et al. Two heads are better than one: both complementary and synchronous strategies facilitate joint action. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.
[12] Kyle B. Reed,et al. Physical Collaboration of Human-Human and Human-Robot Teams , 2008, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.
[13] M. Kawato,et al. Two is better than one: Physical interactions improve motor performance in humans , 2014, Scientific Reports.
[14] P. Mermelstein,et al. Opposite Effects of mGluR1a and mGluR5 Activation on Nucleus Accumbens Medium Spiny Neuron Dendritic Spine Density , 2016, PloS one.
[15] Etienne Burdet,et al. Dissociating Variability and Effort as Determinants of Coordination , 2009, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[16] J. Robinson. AN ITERATIVE METHOD OF SOLVING A GAME , 1951, Classics in Game Theory.
[17] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[18] Helen J. Wall,et al. No evidence for shared representations of task sets in joint task switching , 2016, Psychological Research.
[19] J. Krakauer,et al. A computational neuroanatomy for motor control , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[20] Cordula Vesper,et al. A minimal architecture for joint action , 2010, Neural Networks.
[21] R. Ivry,et al. The coordination of movement: optimal feedback control and beyond , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[22] T. Başar,et al. Dynamic Noncooperative Game Theory , 1982 .
[23] Etienne Burdet,et al. Motion Plan Changes Predictably in Dyadic Reaching , 2016, PloS one.
[24] Daniel A. Braun,et al. Motor coordination: when two have to act as one , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.
[25] Daniel A. Braun,et al. Nash Equilibria in Multi-Agent Motor Interactions , 2009, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[26] H. Bekkering,et al. Joint action: bodies and minds moving together , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[27] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Neural and Computational Mechanisms of Action Processing: Interaction between Visual and Motor Representations , 2015, Neuron.
[28] Robert J. van Beers,et al. How does our motor system determine its learning rate , 2012 .
[29] Robrecht P R D van der Wel,et al. Let the force be with us: dyads exploit haptic coupling for coordination. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] H. Raiffa,et al. GAMES AND DECISIONS; INTRODUCTION AND CRITICAL SURVEY. , 1958 .
[31] Cagatay Basdogan,et al. Supporting Negotiation Behavior with Haptics-Enabled Human-Computer Interfaces , 2012, IEEE Transactions on Haptics.
[32] L. Sentis,et al. The CHAI Libraries , 2003 .
[33] Junya Masumoto,et al. A leader–follower relationship in joint action on a discrete force production task , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.
[34] Michael I. Jordan,et al. An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.
[35] Eric van Damme,et al. Non-Cooperative Games , 2000 .
[36] Peter M. Vishton,et al. Haptically Linked Dyads , 2006, Psychological science.
[37] Daniel A. Braun,et al. The effect of model uncertainty on cooperation in sensorimotor interactions , 2013, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.
[38] E. Burdet,et al. Interpersonal strategies for disturbance attenuation during a rhythmic joint motor action , 2015, Physiology & Behavior.
[39] Etienne Burdet,et al. Haptic communication between humans is tuned by the hard or soft mechanics of interaction , 2018, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[40] Raymond H. Cuijpers,et al. Joint Action: Neurocognitive Mechanisms Supporting Human Interaction , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..
[41] W. Prinz,et al. How two share a task: corepresenting stimulus-response mappings. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] D. Rosenbaum,et al. Etiquette and Effort , 2011, Psychological science.
[43] Robert J. van Beers,et al. How Does Our Motor System Determine Its Learning Rate? , 2012, PloS one.
[44] C. Frith,et al. Follow you, Follow me: Continuous Mutual Prediction and Adaptation in Joint Tapping , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[45] Konrad P. Körding,et al. Uncertainty of Feedback and State Estimation Determines the Speed of Motor Adaptation , 2009, Front. Comput. Neurosci..
[46] Helen J. Wall,et al. Sharing tasks or sharing actions? Evidence from the joint Simon task , 2016, Psychological research.