Cortical regions activated by the subjective sense of perceptual coherence of environmental sounds: A proposal for a neuroscience of intuition

According to the Oxford English Dictionary, intuition is “the ability to understand or know something immediately, without conscious reasoning.” In other words, people continuously, without conscious attention, recognize patterns in the stream of sensations that impinge upon them. The result is a vague perception of coherence, which subsequently biases thought and behavior accordingly. Within the visual domain, research using paradigms with difficult recognition has suggested that the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) serves as a fast detector and predictor of potential content that utilizes coarse facets of the input. To investigate whether the OFC is crucial in biasing task-specific processing, and hence subserves intuitive judgments in various modalities, we used a difficult-recognition paradigm in the auditory domain. Participants were presented with short sequences of distorted, nonverbal, environmental sounds and had to perform a sound categorization task. Imaging results revealed rostral medial OFC activation for such auditory intuitive coherence judgments. By means of a conjunction analysis between the present results and those from a previous study on visual intuitive coherence judgments, the rostral medial OFC was shown to be activated via both modalities. We conclude that rostral OFC activation during intuitive coherence judgments subserves the detection of potential content on the basis of only coarse facets of the input.

[1]  Jesper Andersson,et al.  Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic , 2005, NeuroImage.

[2]  D G Norris,et al.  Reduced power multislice MDEFT imaging , 2000, Journal of magnetic resonance imaging : JMRI.

[3]  R. Turner,et al.  Characterizing Evoked Hemodynamics with fMRI , 1995, NeuroImage.

[4]  D. Pandya,et al.  Architecture and Connections of Cortical Association Areas , 1985 .

[5]  Rolf Reber,et al.  Exploring “fringe” consciousness: The subjective experience of perceptual fluency and its objective bases , 2004, Consciousness and Cognition.

[6]  T. Robbins,et al.  Effects of orbitofrontal, infralimbic and prelimbic cortical lesions on serial spatial reversal learning in the rat , 2007, Behavioural Brain Research.

[7]  John R. Anderson A Spreading Activation Theory of Memory , 1988 .

[8]  S. Epstein The self-concept revisited. Or a theory of a theory. , 1973, The American psychologist.

[9]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Neural Responses during Anticipation of a Primary Taste Reward , 2002, Neuron.

[10]  Gabriele Lohmann,et al.  Bayesian second-level analysis of functional magnetic resonance images , 2003, NeuroImage.

[11]  Nadim Joni Shah,et al.  Neural processes underlying intuitive coherence judgments as revealed by fMRI on a semantic judgment task , 2007, NeuroImage.

[12]  H. Barbas,et al.  Architecture and cortical connections of the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey. , 1992, Advances in neurology.

[13]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Distinct Cortical Pathways for Processing Tool versus Animal Sounds , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  Geoffrey Schoenbaum,et al.  Rapid Associative Encoding in Basolateral Amygdala Depends on Connections with Orbitofrontal Cortex , 2005, Neuron.

[15]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[16]  E. Higgins,et al.  Regulatory Focus and Strategic Inclinations : Promotion and Prevention in Decision-Making , 1997 .

[17]  G. Fricchione Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain , 1995 .

[18]  A. Damasio,et al.  Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.

[19]  Jean-Philippe Thirion,et al.  Image matching as a diffusion process: an analogy with Maxwell's demons , 1998, Medical Image Anal..

[20]  Hervé Abdi,et al.  Signal Detection Theory (SDT) , 2007 .

[21]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment , 2002 .

[22]  Jeffrey R Binder,et al.  Human brain regions involved in recognizing environmental sounds. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[23]  Douglas L. Medin,et al.  Chapter 13 – Concepts and Categorization , 1998 .

[24]  Geoffrey Schoenbaum,et al.  From associations to expectancies: orbitofrontal cortex as gateway between the limbic system and representational memory , 2006 .

[25]  Stefan Koelsch,et al.  Bach Speaks: A Cortical “Language-Network” Serves the Processing of Music , 2002, NeuroImage.

[26]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited—Again , 1995, NeuroImage.

[27]  Kenneth R. Hammond,et al.  Beyond Rationality: The Search for Wisdom in a Troubled Time , 2007 .

[28]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: Individual Differences in Reasoning: Implications for the Rationality Debate? , 2002 .

[29]  Joseph G. Johnson,et al.  Expertise-based differences in search and option-generation strategies. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[30]  M. Roesch,et al.  Orbitofrontal Cortex, Associative Learning, and Expectancies , 2005, Neuron.

[31]  P. Jezzard,et al.  Correction for geometric distortion in echo planar images from B0 field variations , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[32]  R. Turner,et al.  Event-Related fMRI: Characterizing Differential Responses , 1998, NeuroImage.

[33]  M. Roesch,et al.  Orbitofrontal cortex, decision-making and drug addiction , 2006, Trends in Neurosciences.

[34]  J. Parkinson,et al.  Dissociable Contributions of the Human Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex to Incentive Motivation and Goal Selection , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[35]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[36]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Encoding Predictive Reward Value in Human Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[37]  M. Bar The proactive brain: using analogies and associations to generate predictions , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  G Lohmann,et al.  LIPSIA--a new software system for the evaluation of functional magnetic resonance images of the human brain. , 2001, Computerized medical imaging and graphics : the official journal of the Computerized Medical Imaging Society.

[39]  C. Betsch Präferenz für Intuition und Deliberation (PID) , 2004 .

[40]  G. Regehr,et al.  Intuition in the context of discovery , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[41]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  Concepts and Categorization , 2003 .

[42]  Peter Boesiger,et al.  Comparison of fMRI activation as measured with gradient- and spin-echo EPI during visual perception , 2005, NeuroImage.

[43]  D. Pandya,et al.  Frontal lobe connections of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[44]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  What Neuroscience Can Tell about Intuitive Processes in the Context of Perceptual Discovery , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  E. Rolls,et al.  The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology , 2004, Progress in Neurobiology.

[46]  Toralf Mildner,et al.  An Investigation of the Value of Spin-Echo-Based fMRI Using a Stroop Color–Word Matching Task and EPI at 3 T , 2002, NeuroImage.

[47]  T. Pearson,et al.  Direct comparison of the efficacy of intuitive and analytical cognition in expert judgment , 1987, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[48]  M. Kringelbach The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[49]  A. Friederici,et al.  Musical syntax is processed in Broca's area: an MEG study , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  H. Barbas,et al.  Relationship of prefrontal connections to inhibitory systems in superior temporal areas in the rhesus monkey. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[51]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Imaging at high magnetic fields: initial experiences at 4 T. , 1993, Magnetic resonance quarterly.

[52]  Jonathan Berger,et al.  Neural Dynamics of Event Segmentation in Music: Converging Evidence for Dissociable Ventral and Dorsal Networks , 2007, Neuron.

[53]  A. Damasio,et al.  Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[54]  Karl J. Friston Functional and effective connectivity in neuroimaging: A synthesis , 1994 .

[55]  J. Kaas,et al.  Subdivisions of auditory cortex and processing streams in primates. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[56]  H. Plessner,et al.  Intuition in judgment and decision making , 2008 .

[57]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. , 2002 .

[58]  S. Petersen,et al.  Characterizing the Hemodynamic Response: Effects of Presentation Rate, Sampling Procedure, and the Possibility of Ordering Brain Activity Based on Relative Timing , 2000, NeuroImage.

[59]  Peter A. Bandettini,et al.  Detection versus Estimation in Event-Related fMRI: Choosing the Optimal Stimulus Timing , 2002, NeuroImage.

[60]  Sascha Topolinski,et al.  Where there's a will-there's no intuition. The unintentional basis of semantic coherence judgments , 2008 .