Reach corrections toward moving objects are faster than reach corrections toward instantaneously switching targets

[1]  Jude F. Mitchell,et al.  Fast prediction in marmoset reach-to-grasp movements for dynamic prey , 2023, Current Biology.

[2]  E. Brenner,et al.  Tapping on a target: dealing with uncertainty about its position and motion , 2022, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  T. Kizuka,et al.  The Effect of Target Velocity on the Fast Corrective Response during Reaching Movement , 2022, Journal of motor behavior.

[4]  E. Brenner,et al.  Hand movements respond to any motion near the endpoint , 2022, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[5]  E. Brenner,et al.  The influences of target size and recent experience on the vigour of adjustments to ongoing movements , 2022, Experimental Brain Research.

[6]  Jacqueline C. Snow,et al.  Graspability Modulates the Stronger Neural Signature of Motor Preparation for Real Objects vs. Pictures , 2021, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  Jacqueline C. Snow,et al.  The Treachery of Images: How Realism Influences Brain and Behavior , 2021, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[8]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  Voluntary modification of rapid tactile-motor responses during reaching differs from its visuo-motor counterpart , 2020, bioRxiv.

[9]  Robert Volcic,et al.  Grasping adjustments to haptic, visual and visuo-haptic object perturbations are contingent on the sensory modality. , 2019, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Joel Nothman,et al.  SciPy 1.0-Fundamental Algorithms for Scientific Computing in Python , 2019, ArXiv.

[11]  J. Duysens,et al.  Postural responses to target jumps and background motion in a fast pointing task , 2018, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  Jacqueline C. Snow,et al.  Graspable Objects Grab Attention More Than Images Do , 2018, Psychological science.

[13]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Errors in interception can be predicted from errors in perception , 2018, Cortex.

[14]  J. Culham,et al.  Getting a grip on reality: Grasping movements directed to real objects and images rely on dissociable neural representations , 2018, Cortex.

[15]  Chao Gu,et al.  A Trial-by-Trial Window into Sensorimotor Transformations in the Human Motor Periphery , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  Leonie Oostwoud Wijdenes,et al.  Movement Adjustments Have Short Latencies Because There is No Need to Detect Anything. , 2016, Motor control.

[17]  J. Randall Flanagan,et al.  A Rapid Tactile-Motor Reflex Automatically Guides Reaching toward Handheld Objects , 2016, Current Biology.

[18]  Sae Franklin,et al.  Temporal Evolution of Spatial Computations for Visuomotor Control , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Pratik K. Mutha,et al.  The influence of visual target information on the online control of movements , 2015, Vision Research.

[20]  Seiji Ono,et al.  The neuronal basis of on-line visual control in smooth pursuit eye movements , 2015, Vision Research.

[21]  Raoul M. Bongers,et al.  Movements of Individual Digits in Bimanual Prehension Are Coupled into a Grasping Component , 2014, PloS one.

[22]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Analysis of methods to determine the latency of online movement adjustments , 2014, Behavior research methods.

[23]  H. Gomi,et al.  Eye–hand coordination in on-line visuomotor adjustments , 2013, Neuroreport.

[24]  R. Caminiti,et al.  Impairment of online control of hand and eye movements in a monkey model of optic ataxia. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[25]  D Voudouris,et al.  Ultra-fast selection of grasping points. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  Thomas Schenk,et al.  No Dissociation between Perception and Action in Patient DF When Haptic Feedback is Withdrawn , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  E. Brenner,et al.  Fast and fine-tuned corrections when the target of a hand movement is displaced , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Continuous visual control of interception. , 2011, Human movement science.

[29]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  Stimulus‐locked responses on human arm muscles reveal a rapid neural pathway linking visual input to arm motor output , 2010, The European journal of neuroscience.

[30]  J F Kalaska,et al.  Integration of predictive feedforward and sensory feedback signals for online control of visually guided movement. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  Jody C Culham,et al.  Is That within Reach? fMRI Reveals That the Human Superior Parieto-Occipital Cortex Encodes Objects Reachable by the Hand , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Specificity of Reflex Adaptation for Task-Relevant Variability , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Eli Brenner,et al.  Consistent haptic feedback is required but it is not enough for natural reaching to virtual cylinders. , 2008, Human movement science.

[34]  Eli Brenner,et al.  The latency for correcting a movement depends on the visual attribute that defines the target , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  C. V. D. Kamp,et al.  Prehension is really reaching and grasping , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Spatiotemporal Tuning of Rapid Interactions between Visual-Motion Analysis and Reaching Movement , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  Umberto Castiello,et al.  An object-centred reference frame for control of grasping: effects of grasping a distractor object on visuomotor control , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  Shin'ya Nishida,et al.  Large-Field Visual Motion Directly Induces an Involuntary Rapid Manual Following Response , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  R. Krauzlis Recasting the smooth pursuit eye movement system. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  Richard J Krauzlis,et al.  Primacy of spatial information in guiding target selection for pursuit and saccades. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[41]  P. Haggard,et al.  Action and awareness in pointing tasks , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  Anne-Marie Brouwer,et al.  Hitting moving objects: is target speed used in guiding the hand? , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  M. Hayhoe,et al.  In what ways do eye movements contribute to everyday activities? , 2001, Vision Research.

[44]  P. Brown,et al.  Evidence for subcortical involvement in the visual control of human reaching. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  N. Shimizu [Neurology of eye movements]. , 2000, Rinsho shinkeigaku = Clinical neurology.

[46]  M. Desmurget,et al.  An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[47]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Ocular gaze is anchored to the target of an ongoing pointing movement. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  U. Castiello,et al.  Reach to grasp: the response to a simultaneous perturbation of object position and size , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[49]  E. Brenner,et al.  Fast Responses of the Human Hand to Changes in Target Position. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[50]  F A Miles,et al.  Release of fixation for pursuit and saccades in humans: evidence for shared inputs acting on different neural substrates. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[51]  E Brenner,et al.  Perception and action are based on the same visual information: distinction between position and velocity. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  C. Bard,et al.  Directional control of rapid arm movements: the role of the kinetic visual feedback system. , 1993, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[53]  M. Goodale,et al.  Grasping versus pointing and the differential use of visual feedback , 1993 .

[54]  M. Gentilucci,et al.  Temporal coupling between transport and grasp components during prehension movements: effects of visual perturbation , 1992, Behavioural Brain Research.

[55]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Automatic control during hand reaching at undetected two-dimensional target displacements. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  B. Alstermark,et al.  Effect of different spinal cord lesions on visually guided switching of target-reaching in cats , 1987, Neuroscience Research.

[57]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Large adjustments in visually guided reaching do not depend on vision of the hand or perception of target displacement , 1986, Nature.

[58]  J. Soechting,et al.  Modification of trajectory of a pointing movement in response to a change in target location. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[59]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Spatial trajectories and reaction times of aimed movements: effects of practice, uncertainty, and change in target location. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[60]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[61]  G. D'mello,et al.  A conveyor belt task for assessing visuo-motor coordination in the marmoset (Callithrix jacchus): Effects of diazepam, chlorpromazine, pentobarbital and d-amphetamine , 2004, Psychopharmacology.

[62]  M. Goodale,et al.  Visual control of reaching movements without vision of the limb , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[63]  E. Brenner,et al.  Fast corrections of movements with a computer mouse. , 2003, Spatial vision.

[64]  B. Day,et al.  Voluntary modification of automatic arm movements evoked by motion of a visual target , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.