Directional Anisotropies Reveal a Functional Segregation of Visual Motion Processing for Perception and Action

Human exhibits an anisotropy in direction perception: discrimination is superior when motion is around horizontal or vertical rather than diagonal axes. In contrast to the consistent directional anisotropy in perception, we found only small idiosyncratic anisotropies in smooth pursuit eye movements, a motor action requiring accurate discrimination of visual motion direction. Both pursuit and perceptual direction discrimination rely on signals from the middle temporal visual area (MT), yet analysis of multiple measures of MT neuronal responses in the macaque failed to provide evidence of a directional anisotropy. We conclude that MT represents different motion directions uniformly, and subsequent processing creates a directional anisotropy in pathways unique to perception. Our data support the hypothesis that, at least for visual motion, perception and action are guided by inputs from separate sensory streams. The directional anisotropy of perception appears to originate after the two streams have segregated and downstream from area MT.

[1]  Richard T. Dyde,et al.  Two illusions of perceived orientation: one fools all of the people some of the time; the other fools all of the people all of the time , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[3]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  V. Franz,et al.  Action does not resist visual illusions , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  M. Perenin,et al.  Optic ataxia: a specific disruption in visuomotor mechanisms. I. Different aspects of the deficit in reaching for objects. , 1988, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  T. Albright Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  William T. Newsome,et al.  Cortical microstimulation influences perceptual judgements of motion direction , 1990, Nature.

[8]  S. Zeki Functional specialisation in the visual cortex of the rhesus monkey , 1978, Nature.

[9]  David C. Burr,et al.  Separate visual representations for perception and action revealed by saccadic eye movements , 2001, Current Biology.

[10]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Visual motion processing and sensory-motor integration for smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  Nicholas J. Priebe,et al.  Constraints on the source of short-term motion adaptation in macaque area MT. I. the role of input and intrinsic mechanisms. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  E. L. Keller,et al.  Generation of smooth-pursuit eye movements: neuronal mechanisms and pathways , 1991, Neuroscience Research.

[13]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. III. Interaction with full-field visual stimulation. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Relationship between eye acceleration and retinal image velocity during foveal smooth pursuit in man and monkey. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  T. R. Jordan,et al.  Perception and action in 'visual form agnosia'. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  R Sekuler,et al.  Models of stimulus uncertainty in motion perception. , 1980, Psychological review.

[17]  W. Newsome,et al.  Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects on direction discrimination performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[18]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Neil A. Macmillan,et al.  Detection Theory: A User's Guide , 1991 .

[20]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[21]  Stephen G Lisberger,et al.  Gain control in human smooth-pursuit eye movements. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Modulation of pursuit eye movements by stimulation of cortical areas MT and MST. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  H. Komatsu,et al.  Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. I. Localization and visual properties of neurons. , 1988, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Visual recognition impairment following medial thalamic lesions in monkeys , 1983, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[26]  P. Carpenter,et al.  Frames of reference for allocating attention to space: Evidence from the neglect syndrome , 1990, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  Stephen J Heinen,et al.  Human smooth pursuit direction discrimination , 1999, Vision Research.

[28]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Saccadic eye movements towards stimuli triggered by prior saccades , 1976, Vision Research.

[29]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[30]  L. P. O'Keefe,et al.  Effects of Early-Onset Artificial Strabismus on Pursuit Eye Movements and on Neuronal Responses in Area MT of Macaque Monkeys , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  W. Newsome,et al.  Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[32]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Analysis of visual behavior , 1982 .

[33]  Brent R. Beutter,et al.  Human motion perception and smooth eye movements slow similar directional biases for elongated apertures , 1998, Vision Research.

[34]  D. W. Heeley,et al.  Directional acuity for drifting plaids , 1992, Vision Research.

[35]  G D Lewen,et al.  Reproducibility and Variability in Neural Spike Trains , 1997, Science.

[36]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  Sensitivity of smooth eye movement to small differences in target velocity , 1987, Vision Research.

[37]  Richard O. Duda,et al.  Pattern classification and scene analysis , 1974, A Wiley-Interscience publication.

[38]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Impairments of visual object transforms in monkeys. , 1984, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Different responses to small visual errors during initiation and maintenance of smooth-pursuit eye movements in monkeys. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  B. L. Gros,et al.  Anisotropies in visual motion perception: a fresh look. , 1998, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[41]  K. H. Britten,et al.  A relationship between behavioral choice and the visual responses of neurons in macaque MT , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.

[42]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.

[43]  R J Krauzlis,et al.  Effects of directional expectations on motion perception and pursuit eye movements , 2001, Visual Neuroscience.

[44]  Nicola Bruno,et al.  When does action resist visual illusions? , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  A. Milner Perception and action in visual form agnosia , 2000 .