Not Quite as Grown-Up as We Like to Think

Greater continuity in cognition between children and adults may exist than is usually appreciated. It was thought that after 3 to 4 years of age, the problem in switching on the dimensional-change card-sort task disappears. We show here, however, that if speed is used as the dependent measure, the effect of the first dimension is evident even in adults. Adults, like preschoolers, show difficulty in switching from a block of sorting by color or shape to a block of sorting by the other dimension. Notably, performance throughout the session was affected by the first dimension by which stimuli were sorted. We hypothesize that perhaps adults never fully outgrow any of the cognitive and perceptual biases of infancy and early childhood. Other examples of such biases that appear to still be present in adults are discussed. Conversely, the assumption that the optimal dependent measure for adults is the most sensitive measure for children is questioned.

[1]  J. Pratt,et al.  Symbolic control of visual attention: The role of working memory and attentional control settings. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  A. Diamond,et al.  Executive Functioning in Preschoolers: Reducing the Inhibitory Demands of the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task , 2004, Developmental neuropsychology.

[3]  B. Hommel,et al.  Task-switching and long-term priming: Role of episodic stimulus–task bindings in task-shift costs , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[4]  U. Mayr,et al.  Is there an age deficit in the selection of mental sets? , 2001 .

[5]  A. Gopnik,et al.  Duck or rabbit? Reversing ambiguous figures and understanding ambiguous representations. , 2001 .

[6]  Y. Munakata,et al.  All Together Now: When Dissociations Between Knowledge and Action Disappear , 2001, Psychological science.

[7]  P. Zelazo,et al.  An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them ☆ , 1996 .

[8]  F. J. Langdon,et al.  The Child's Conception of Space , 1967 .

[9]  Ulrich Müller,et al.  The development of executive function in early childhood. , 1991, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[10]  Paul Bloom,et al.  Children Are Cursed , 2003, Psychological science.

[11]  D. Barr,et al.  Limits on theory of mind use in adults , 2003, Cognition.

[12]  E. Bialystok,et al.  Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. , 2004, Developmental science.

[13]  S. Barrett,et al.  Developmental changes in attentional skills: the effect of irrelevant variations on encoding and response selection. , 1988, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[14]  Josef Perner,et al.  Disentangling dimensions in the dimensional change card-sorting task. , 2005, Developmental science.

[15]  Ivan Mervielde,et al.  The Need for Closure and the Spontaneous Use of Complex and Simple Cognitive Structures , 2003, The Journal of social psychology.

[16]  Jacob Cohen Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences , 1969, The SAGE Encyclopedia of Research Design.

[17]  R. Hastie,et al.  Hindsight: Biased judgments of past events after the outcomes are known. , 1990 .

[18]  M. Schmitter-Edgecombe,et al.  Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks following severe closed-head injury. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[19]  S. Los,et al.  On the origin of mixing costs: exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials , 1996 .

[20]  Philip David Zelazo,et al.  Children's rule use: Representation, reflection, and cognitive control , 1997 .

[21]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  Josef Perner,et al.  Theory of mind and self-control: more than a common problem of inhibition. , 2002, Child development.

[23]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  I knew it would happen: Remembered probabilities of once—future things , 1975 .

[24]  Colin M. Macleod Training on integrated versus separated Stroop tasks: The progression of interference and facilitation , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[25]  A. Allport,et al.  Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs” , 2000, Psychological research.

[26]  H. Wimmer,et al.  Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit , 1987 .

[27]  A. Churchland,et al.  Early developments in the ability to understand the relation between stimulus and reward. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[28]  Ulrich Müller,et al.  The development of executive function in early childhood: I. The development of executive function. , 2003 .

[29]  Carey K. Morewedge,et al.  Perspective taking in children and adults: Equivalent egocentrism but differential correction , 2004 .

[30]  Natasha Z. Kirkham,et al.  ARTICLE WITH PEER COMMENTARIES AND RESPONSE Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task , 2003 .

[31]  Pamela J. Hinds,et al.  The curse of expertise: The effects of expertise and debiasing methods on prediction of novice performance. , 1999 .

[32]  J. Flavell,et al.  Development of knowledge about the appearance-reality distinction. , 1986, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[33]  Daniel Reisberg,et al.  What an image depicts depends on what an image means , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[34]  J-M Hopf,et al.  Dynamics of feature binding during object-selective attention , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[35]  P. Lachenbruch Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.) , 1989 .

[36]  Raymond S. Nickerson,et al.  How We Know—and Sometimes Misjudge—What Others Know: Imputing One's Own Knowledge to Others , 1999 .

[37]  A. Diamond,et al.  Early success in using the relation between stimuli and rewards to deduce an abstract rule: perceived physical connection is key. , 2003, Developmental psychology.