Prospective memory in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND): The neuropsychological dynamics of time monitoring

Strategic monitoring during a delay interval is theorized to be an essential feature of time-based prospective memory (TB PM), the cognitive architecture of which is thought to rely heavily on frontostriatal systems and executive functions. This hypothesis was examined in 55 individuals with HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) and 108 seronegative comparison participants who were administered the Memory for Intentions Screening Test (MIST), during which time-monitoring (clock-checking) behavior was measured. Results revealed a significant interaction between HAND group and the frequency of clock checking, in which individuals with HAND checked the clock significantly less often than the comparison group across the TB PM retention intervals of the MIST. Subsequent analyses in the HAND sample revealed that the frequency of clocking checking was positively related to overall TB performance, as well as to standard clinical measures of retrospective memory and verbal fluency. These findings add support to a growing body of research elucidating TB PM's reliance on strategic monitoring processes dependent upon intact frontostriatal systems. HIV-associated TB strategic time-monitoring deficits may manifest in poorer functioning outcomes, including medication nonadherence and dependence in activities of daily living. Future research is needed to further delineate the cognitive mechanisms underlying strategic time monitoring in order to advise rehabilitation strategies for reducing HAND-related TB PM deficits.

[1]  Michael J. Taylor,et al.  Semantic clustering inefficiency in HIV-associated dementia. , 2007, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[2]  I. Grant,et al.  Is prospective memory a dissociable cognitive function in HIV infection? , 2010, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[3]  M. Lorr,et al.  Manual for the Profile of Mood States , 1971 .

[4]  T. B. Üstün,et al.  The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) , 2004, International journal of methods in psychiatric research.

[5]  I. Grant,et al.  Aging, Prospective Memory, and Health-Related Quality of Life in HIV Infection , 2012, AIDS and Behavior.

[6]  S. Raskin Memory for Intentions Screening Test: Psychometric Properties and Clinical Evidence , 2009, Brain Impairment.

[7]  I. Grant,et al.  Longer ongoing task delay intervals exacerbate prospective memory deficits in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) , 2012, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[8]  Matthias Kliegel,et al.  Complex prospective memory: development across the lifespan and the role of task interruption. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[9]  Oliver Speck,et al.  Perfusion MRI detects rCBF abnormalities in early stages of HIV–cognitive motor complex , 2000, Neurology.

[10]  J Duncan,et al.  Rehabilitation of executive functioning: An experimental–clinical validation of Goal Management Training , 2000, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[11]  S. L. Richardson,et al.  Aging and prospective memory: examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[12]  I. Grant,et al.  Interrater Reliability of Clinical Ratings and Neurocognitive Diagnoses in HIV , 2004, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[13]  Carla J. Reicks,et al.  The impact of HIV-associated neuropsychological impairment on everyday functioning , 2004, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[14]  I. Grant,et al.  Frequency and predictors of self-reported prospective memory complaints in individuals infected with HIV. , 2007, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[15]  Paul E. Gilbert,et al.  An examination of the age-prospective memory paradox in HIV-infected adults , 2011, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[16]  I. Grant,et al.  Markers of Macrophage Activation and Axonal Injury are Associated With Prospective Memory in HIV-1 Disease , 2006, Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.

[17]  I. Grant,et al.  Timing is everything: Antiretroviral nonadherence is associated with impairment in time-based prospective memory , 2009, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[18]  M. Maschke,et al.  Structural gray and white matter changes in patients with HIV , 2011, Journal of Neurology.

[19]  Sergio Della Sala,et al.  Working memory in event‐ and time‐based prospective memory tasks: Effects of secondary demand and age , 2004 .

[20]  R. Reitan,et al.  The Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery: Theory and clinical interpretation , 1993 .

[21]  David Shum,et al.  Performance of schizophrenia patients on time-, event-, and activity-based prospective memory tasks. , 2004, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[22]  I. Grant,et al.  Memory-Based Strategies for Antiretroviral Medication Management: An Evaluation of Clinical Predictors, Adherence Behavior Awareness, and Effectiveness , 2012, AIDS and Behavior.

[23]  J. Becker,et al.  Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders , 2007, Neurology.

[24]  K. Boone,et al.  A meta-analysis of the neuropsychological sequelae of HIV infection , 2002, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[25]  Joanna Jacobus,et al.  Characteristics of prospective memory deficits in HIV-seropositive substance-dependent individuals: Preliminary observations , 2007, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[26]  R. R. Hunt,et al.  The cost of event-based prospective memory: salient target events. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  I. Grant,et al.  HIV-associated prospective memory impairment increases risk of dependence in everyday functioning. , 2008, Neuropsychology.

[28]  Tom Manly,et al.  Rehabilitation of executive dysfunction following brain injury: “Content-free” cueing improves everyday prospective memory performance , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  I. Grant,et al.   Psychometric Characteristics of the Memory for Intentions Screening Test , 2008, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[30]  L. Kvavilashvili,et al.  Is time-based prospective remembering mediated by self-initiated rehearsals? Role of incidental cues, ongoing activity, age, and motivation. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[31]  D. Delis,et al.  The myth of testing construct validity using factor analysis or correlations with normal or mixed clinical populations: Lessons from memory assessment , 2003, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[32]  R. Marsh,et al.  Task interference in time-based, event-based, and dual intention prospective memory conditions☆ , 2005 .

[33]  Arnold J. Wilkins,et al.  Remembering to do things: A theoretical framework and an illustrative experiment , 1982 .

[34]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Aging and prospective memory: examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes. , 1995 .

[35]  A. Tröster,et al.  Action (verb) fluency: Test–retest reliability, normative standards, and construct validity , 2005, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[36]  C. Caltagirone,et al.  Prospective and retrospective components in the memory for actions to be performed in patients with severe closed-head injury , 2004, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[37]  G. Winocur,et al.  Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: evidence from younger and older healthy adults. , 1997, Neuropsychology.

[38]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Prospective Memory , 2005 .

[39]  M. Bondi,et al.  Intraindividual variability in HIV infection: evidence for greater neurocognitive dispersion in older HIV seropositive adults. , 2011, Neuropsychology.

[40]  Terry L. Jernigan,et al.  HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders before and during the era of combination antiretroviral therapy: differences in rates, nature, and predictors , 2010, Journal of NeuroVirology.

[41]  I. Grant,et al.  Verbal fluency in HIV infection: A meta-analytic review , 2006, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[42]  Nathan S. Rose,et al.  A process-model based approach to prospective memory impairment in Parkinson's disease , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  K. Goodkin,et al.  Cognitive Effects of HIV-1 Infection , 2000, CNS Spectrums.

[44]  I. Grant,et al.  Prospective memory in HIV infection: is "remembering to remember" a unique predictor of self-reported medication management? , 2008, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[45]  D. Moore,et al.  Cliniconeuropathologic correlates of human immunodeficiency virus in the era of antiretroviral therapy , 2009, Journal of NeuroVirology.

[46]  I. Grant,et al.  Prospective Memory and Antiretroviral Medication Non-Adherence in HIV: An Analysis of Ongoing Task Delay Length Using the Memory for Intentions Screening Test , 2012, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[47]  Robert K. Heaton,et al.  Prospective Memory in HIV-1 Infection , 2006, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[48]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: a multiprocess framework , 2000 .

[49]  Morris Moscovitch,et al.  Cognitive resources and dual-task interference effects at retrieval in normal people: The role of the frontal lobes and medial temporal cortex. , 1994 .

[50]  M. Kliegel,et al.  Clinical neuropsychology of prospective memory , 2008 .

[51]  I. Grant,et al.  Semantic cueing improves category verbal fluency in persons living with HIV infection. , 2012, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[52]  W. Sturm,et al.  Neuropsychological assessment , 2007, Journal of Neurology.

[53]  Linda Chang,et al.  Additive effects of HIV and chronic methamphetamine use on brain metabolite abnormalities. , 2005, The American journal of psychiatry.

[54]  C. Caltagirone,et al.  Prospective memory impairment in individuals with Parkinson's disease. , 2008, Neuropsychology.

[55]  C. Fennema-Notestine,et al.  Combined effects of aging and HIV infection on semantic verbal fluency: A view of the cortical hypothesis through the lens of clustering and switching , 2012, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[56]  Erica Weber,et al.  Cognitive mechanisms of switching in HIV-associated category fluency deficits , 2008, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[57]  M. Kliegel,et al.  The role of dual-task and task-switch in prospective memory: Behavioural data and neural correlates , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[58]  I. Grant,et al.  Are time- and event-based prospective memory comparably affected in HIV infection? , 2011, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[59]  T. Mäntylä,et al.  Time monitoring and executive functioning in children and adults. , 2007, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[60]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Multiple processes in prospective memory retrieval: factors determining monitoring versus spontaneous retrieval. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[61]  M. Kinsbourne,et al.  Time estimation by patients with frontal lesions and by Korsakoff amnesics , 2000, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.