Structure, Motion, and Preschoolers' Perceptions of Social Causality

Children 3,4, and 5 years old provided free-response descriptions of one of four short video displays. These consisted of either the original Heider and Simmel (1944) animated film or one of three versions of the film that had been altered with a digital special-effects generator. In the altered versions, (a) dynamic aspects of the original film were disrupted but structural properties were left intact, (b) structural aspects of the film were disrupted but dynamic properties were preserved, or (c) both structural and dynamic properties of the film were disrupted. Analyses of children's descriptions indicated that their propensity to describe the original display in anthropomorphic terms, like adults', is based on the patterns of motion that characterize the film.

[1]  Stephen J. Misovich,et al.  Effects of Disruption of Structure and Motion on Perceptions of Social Causality , 1992 .

[2]  D. Berry What can a moving face tell us? , 1990, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  J. Aronoff,et al.  The recognition of threatening facial stimuli. , 1988, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[4]  D. Berry,et al.  Child and Adult Sensitivity to Gender Information in Patterns of Facial Motion , 1991 .

[5]  D. Premack,et al.  The perception of intention. , 1989, Science.

[6]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[7]  D R Proffitt,et al.  Perception of biomechanical motions by infants: implementation of various processing constraints. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  R. Gelman First Principles Organize Attention to and Learning About Relevant Data: Number and the Animate‐Inanimate Distinction as Examples , 1990 .

[9]  J. N. Bassili Temporal and spatial contingencies in the perception of social events , 1976 .

[10]  D. Premack The infant's theory of self-propelled objects , 1990, Cognition.

[11]  S. Stavros Valenti,et al.  Social Affordances and Interaction I: Introduction , 1991 .

[12]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[13]  G. Ginsburg,et al.  The Ecological Perception Debate: An Affordance of the Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour , 1990 .

[14]  J. Piaget,et al.  The Child's Conception of the World , 1971 .

[15]  Michael D. Berzonsky Child Animism: Situational Influences and Individual Differences , 1988 .

[16]  On the Basicity of Social Perception Cues: Developmental Evidence for Adult Processes? , 1986 .

[17]  J. N. Bassili Facial motion in the perception of faces and of emotional expression. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  WHAT PRESCHOOLERS KNOW ABOUT ANIMATE AND INANIMATE OBJECTS , 1983 .

[19]  A. Pick,et al.  Intermodal perception of happy and angry expressive behaviors by seven-month-old infants. , 1992, Child development.

[20]  F. Heider,et al.  An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .

[21]  Stephen J. Misovich,et al.  Quantized displays of human movement: A methodological alternative to the point-light display , 1991 .

[22]  Lester M. Hyman,et al.  Which are the stimuli in facial displays of anger and happiness? Configurational bases of emotion recognition. , 1992 .

[23]  F. Keil,et al.  Early differentiation of causal mechanisms appropriate to biological and nonbiological kinds. , 1991, Child development.