Information encoding and the responses of single neurons in the primate temporal visual cortex.

1. The possibility of temporal encoding in the spike trains of single neurons recorded in the temporal lobe visual cortical areas of rhesus macaques was analyzed with the use of principal component and information theory analyses of smoothed spike trains. The neurons analyzed had responses selective for faces. 2. Provided that a correction was applied to earlier methods of principal component analysis used for neuronal spike trains, it was shown that the first principal component provides by a great extent the most information, with the second and third adding only small proportions (on average 18.8 and 8.4%, respectively). 3. It was shown that the magnitude of the second and higher principal components is even smaller if the spike train analysis is started after the onset of the neuronal response, instead of before the neuronal response has started. This suggests that variations in response latency are at least a part of what is reflected by the second and higher principal components. 4. The first principal component was correlated with the mean firing rate of the neurons. The second and higher principal components reflected at least partly the onset properties of the neuronal responses, such as response latency differences between the stimuli. 5. A considerable proportion of the information available from principal components 1-3 is available in the firing rate of the neuron. 6. Periods of the firing rate of as little as 50 or even 20 ms are sufficient to give a reasonable estimate of the firing rate of the neuron. 7. Information theory analysis showed that in short epochs (e.g., 50 ms) the information available from the firing rate can be as high, on average, as 84.4% of that available from the firing rate calculated over 400 ms, and 52.0% of that available from principal components 1-3 in the 400-ms period. It was also found that 44.0% of the information calculated from the first three principal components is available in the firing rates calculated over epochs as short as 20 ms. 8. More information was available near the start of the neuronal response, and the information available from short epochs became less later in the neuronal response. 9. Taken together, these analyses provide evidence that a short period of firing taken close to the start of the neuronal response provides a reasonable proportion of the total information that would be available if a long period of neuronal firing (e.g., 400 ms) were utilized to extract it, even if temporal encoding were used.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)

[1]  A. W. Macrae,et al.  On calculating unbiased information measures. , 1971 .

[2]  Keinosuke Fukunaga,et al.  Introduction to Statistical Pattern Recognition , 1972 .

[3]  E. T. Rolls,et al.  Hypothalamic neuronal responses associated with the sight of food , 1976, Brain Research.

[4]  E. Rolls,et al.  The latency of activation of neurones in the lateral hypothalamus and substantia innominata during feeding in the monkey , 1979, Brain Research.

[5]  E. Rolls,et al.  Selectivity between faces in the responses of a population of neurons in the cortex in the superior temporal sulcus of the monkey , 1985, Brain Research.

[6]  L. Optican,et al.  Temporal encoding of two-dimensional patterns by single units in primate inferior temporal cortex. III. Information theoretic analysis. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  E. Rolls,et al.  Functional subdivisions of the temporal lobe neocortex , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  H. Spitzer,et al.  Temporal encoding of two-dimensional patterns by single units in primate inferior temporal cortex. I. Response characteristics. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  B J Richmond,et al.  Temporal encoding of two-dimensional patterns by single units in primate inferior temporal cortex. II. Quantification of response waveform. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  E. Rolls Functions of neuronal networks in the hippocampus and neocortex in memory , 1989 .

[11]  B J Richmond,et al.  Temporal encoding of two-dimensional patterns by single units in primate primary visual cortex. II. Information transmission. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  E. Rolls,et al.  Gustatory responses of single neurons in the caudolateral orbitofrontal cortex of the macaque monkey. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  E. Rolls,et al.  Allocentric and egocentric spatial information processing in the hippocampal formation of the behaving primate , 1991, Psychobiology.

[14]  E. Rolls Neural organization of higher visual functions , 1991, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[15]  E T Rolls,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying face processing within and beyond the temporal cortical visual areas. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[16]  James L. McGaugh,et al.  Brain Organization and Memory: Cells, Systems, and Circuits , 1992 .

[17]  W. Singer,et al.  Temporal coding in the visual cortex: new vistas on integration in the nervous system , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[18]  M. Tovée,et al.  Oscillatory activity is not evident in the primate temporal visual cortex with static stimuli , 1992, Neuroreport.

[19]  L. Optican,et al.  Role of inferior temporal neurons in visual memory. I. Temporal encoding of information about visual images, recalled images, and behavioral context. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  A. Treves Mean-field analysis of neuronal spike dynamics , 1993 .