Choosing between Small, Likely Rewards and Large, Unlikely Rewards Activates Inferior and Orbital Prefrontal Cortex

Patients sustaining lesions of the orbital prefrontal cortex (PFC) exhibit marked impairments in the performance of laboratory-based gambling, or risk-taking, tasks, suggesting that this part of the human PFC contributes to decision-making cognition. However, to date, little is known about the particular regions of the orbital cortex that participate in this function. In the present study, eight healthy volunteers were scanned, using H2150 PET technology, while performing a novel computerized risk-taking task. The task involved predicting which of two mutually exclusive outcomes would occur, but critically, the larger reward (and penalty) was associated with choice of the least likely outcome, whereas the smallest reward (and penalty) was associated with choice of the most likely outcome. Resolving these “conflicting” decisions was associated with three distinct foci of regional cerebral blood flow increase within the right inferior and orbital PFC: laterally, in the anterior part of the middle frontal gyrus [Brodmann area 10 (BA 10)], medially, in the orbital gyrus (BA 11), and posteriorly, in the anterior portion of the inferior frontal gyrus (BA 47). By contrast, increases in the degree of conflict inherent in these decisions was associated with only limited changes in activity within orbital PFC and the anterior cingulate cortex. These results suggest that decision making recruits neural activity from multiple regions of the inferior PFC that receive information from a diverse set of cortical and limbic inputs, and that the contribution of the orbitofrontal regions may involve processing changes in reward-related information.

[1]  M. Cole The Frontal Granular Cortex and Behavior. , 1964 .

[2]  T. Powell,et al.  An anatomical study of converging sensory pathways within the cerebral cortex of the monkey. , 1970, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  M. Mishkin,et al.  Limbic lesions and the problem of stimulus--reinforcement associations. , 1972, Experimental neurology.

[4]  J. Glowinski,et al.  Dopaminergic Terminals in the Rat Cortex , 1973, Science.

[5]  M Linnoila,et al.  Low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentration differentiates impulsive from nonimpulsive violent behavior. , 1983, Life sciences.

[6]  A. Damasio,et al.  Severe disturbance of higher cognition after bilateral frontal lobe ablation: Patient EVR , 1986 .

[7]  D. C. Howell Statistical Methods for Psychology , 1987 .

[8]  G. Di Chiara,et al.  Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. , 1988, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[9]  F. Bloom,et al.  Cellular and molecular mechanisms of drug dependence. , 1988, Science.

[10]  H. Barbas Anatomic organization of basoventral and mediodorsal visual recipient prefrontal regions in the rhesus monkey , 1988, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[11]  R. Wise,et al.  Brain dopamine and reward. , 1989, Annual review of psychology.

[12]  D. Pandya,et al.  Architecture and intrinsic connections of the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey , 1989, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[13]  A. Damasio,et al.  Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[14]  A. Damasio,et al.  Preserved access and processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage , 1991, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  C. Geula,et al.  Cytoarchitecture and neural afferents of orbitofrontal cortex in the brain of the monkey , 1992, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[16]  C. Olson,et al.  Functional heterogeneity in cingulate cortex: the anterior executive and posterior evaluative regions. , 1992, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  J. Fuster Frontal lobes , 1993, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[18]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Detection of Thirty-Second Cognitive Activations in Single Subjects with Positron Emission Tomography: A New Low-Dose H215O Regional Cerebral Blood Flow Three-Dimensional Imaging Technique , 1993, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[19]  E. Rolls,et al.  Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[20]  A. Thierry,et al.  Motor and Cognitive Functions of the Prefrontal Cortex , 1994, Research and Perspectives in Neurosciences.

[21]  J. Price,et al.  Architectonic subdivision of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey , 1994, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[22]  A. Damasio,et al.  Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex , 1994, Cognition.

[23]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  The Issue of Memory in the Study of Prefrontal Function , 1994 .

[24]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[25]  J. Price,et al.  Limbic connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[26]  M. Petrides,et al.  Functional Organization of the Human Frontal Cortex for Mnemonic Processing. , 1995, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[27]  J. Price,et al.  Sensory and premotor connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of macaque monkeys , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[28]  Richard Coppola,et al.  Physiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: A positron emission tomography study , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  E. Lynd-Balta,et al.  The orbital and medial prefrontal circuit through the primate basal ganglia , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[30]  H. Damasio,et al.  Human Neuroanatomy Relevant to Decision-Making , 1996 .

[31]  E. Rolls,et al.  The Orbitofrontal Cortex , 2019 .

[32]  Hiroshi Fukuda,et al.  Functional anatomy of GO/NO-GO discrimination and response selection — a PET study in man , 1996, Brain Research.

[33]  A. Damasio The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[34]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory processing within the lateral frontal cortex: a positron emission tomography study. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[35]  J. Jonides,et al.  Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[36]  Angela C. Roberts Executive and cognitive functions of the prefrontal cortex - Introduction , 1996 .

[37]  A. Damasio,et al.  Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts , 1996, Nature.

[39]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[40]  P. Goldman-Rakic The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[41]  M. Petrides,et al.  Specialized systems for the processing of mnemonic information within the primate frontal cortex. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[42]  A. Damasio,et al.  Deciding Advantageously Before Knowing the Advantageous Strategy , 1997, Science.

[43]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Transient and sustained activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory , 1997, Nature.

[44]  P. Goldman-Rakic The prefrontal landscape: implications of functional architecture for understanding human mentation and the central executive. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[45]  B. Kushner Descartes' error. , 1998, Journal of AAPOS : the official publication of the American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus.

[46]  Y. Miyashita,et al.  No‐go dominant brain activity in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging , 1998, The European journal of neuroscience.

[47]  H. Damasio,et al.  Dissociation Of Working Memory from Decision Making within the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  R. Passingham,et al.  The Preparation, Execution and Suppression of Copied Movements in the Human Brain , 1996 .

[49]  C. Frith,et al.  Orbitofrontal cortex is activated during breaches of expectation in tasks of visual attention , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  T. Robbins,et al.  Specific cognitive deficits in mild frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[51]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Dissociable neural responses associated with reward, punishment and risk-taking , 1999 .

[52]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociable Deficits in the Decision-Making Cognition of Chronic Amphetamine Abusers, Opiate Abusers, Patients with Focal Damage to Prefrontal Cortex, and Tryptophan-Depleted Normal Volunteers: Evidence for Monoaminergic Mechanisms , 1999, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[53]  Gregory P. Lee,et al.  Different Contributions of the Human Amygdala and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex to Decision-Making , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.