Tactile acuity in the blind: a psychophysical study using a two-dimensional angle discrimination task

Growing evidence suggests that blind subjects outperform the sighted on certain tactile discrimination tasks depending on cutaneous inputs. The purpose of this study was to compare the performance of blind (n = 14) and sighted (n = 15) subjects in a haptic angle discrimination task, depending on both cutaneous and proprioceptive feedback. Subjects actively scanned their right index finger over pairs of two-dimensional (2-D) angles (standard 90°; comparison 91–103°), identifying the larger one. Two exploratory strategies were tested: arm straight or arm flexed at the elbow so that joint movement was, respectively, mainly proximal (shoulder) or distal (wrist, finger). The mean discrimination thresholds for the sighted subjects (vision occluded) were similar for both exploratory strategies (5.7 and 5.8°, respectively). Exploratory strategy likewise did not modify threshold in the blind subjects (proximal 4.3°; distal 4.9°), but thresholds were on average lower than for the sighted subjects. A between-group comparison indicated that blind subjects had significantly lower thresholds than did the sighted subjects, but only for the proximal condition. The superior performance of the blind subjects likely represents heightened sensitivity to haptic inputs in response to visual deprivation, which, in these subjects, occurred prior to 14 years of age.

[1]  R. Zatorre,et al.  A Functional Neuroimaging Study of Sound Localization: Visual Cortex Activity Predicts Performance in Early-Blind Individuals , 2005, PLoS biology.

[2]  Arthur C. Grant,et al.  Tactile perception in blind Braille readers: A psychophysical study of acuity and hyperacuity using gratings and dot patterns , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[3]  K. Sathian,et al.  Multisensory cortical processing of object shape and its relation to mental imagery , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[4]  Franco Lepore,et al.  Early- and Late-Onset Blind Individuals Show Supra-Normal Auditory Abilities in Far-Space , 2004, Current Biology.

[5]  Ingrid M. Kanics,et al.  Tactile Acuity is Enhanced in Blindness , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  L. Merabet,et al.  The plastic human brain cortex. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.

[7]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  H Burton,et al.  Reading embossed capital letters: An fMRI study in blind and sighted individuals , 2006, Human brain mapping.

[9]  Y. Lamarre,et al.  Haptic discrimination of object shape in humans: contribution of cutaneous and proprioceptive inputs , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  C. Elaine Chapman,et al.  Haptic shape discrimination in humans: insight into haptic frames of reference , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  T. Hendler,et al.  Visuo-haptic object-related activation in the ventral visual pathway , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  Norihiro Sadato,et al.  How the Blind “See” Braille: Lessons From Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging , 2005, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[14]  P W Davidson,et al.  Haptic judgments of curvature by blind and sighted humans. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[15]  H. Burton,et al.  Cortical activity to vibrotactile stimulation: An fMRI study in blind and sighted individuals , 2004, Human brain mapping.

[16]  K. Yuen,et al.  Correction: Design of Wide-Spectrum Inhibitors Targeting Coronavirus Main Proteases , 2005, PLoS Biology.

[17]  C. E. Chapman,et al.  Haptic discrimination of object shape in humans: two-dimensional angle discrimination , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  Ian M. L. Hunter Tactile-Kinaesthetic Perception of Straightness in Blind and Sighted Humans , 1954 .

[19]  Y. Mauss,et al.  Blindness and brain plasticity: contribution of mental imagery? An fMRI study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[20]  Manabu Honda,et al.  Critical Period for Cross-Modal Plasticity in Blind Humans: A Functional MRI Study , 2002, NeuroImage.

[21]  Xiaoping Hu,et al.  Activity and effective connectivity of parietal and occipital cortical regions during haptic shape perception , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Alexia for Braille following bilateral occipital stroke in an early blind woman , 2000, NeuroReport.

[23]  Herbert L. Pick,et al.  A developmental study of tactual discrimination in blind and sighted children and adults , 1966 .

[24]  M. Hallett,et al.  Period of susceptibility for cross‐modal plasticity in the blind , 1999, Annals of neurology.

[25]  Myriam Levy,et al.  Haptic discrimination of two-dimensional angles: influence of exploratory strategy , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  J. Ponsford Tactile spatial resolution in blind Braille readers , 2000, Neurology.

[27]  J. Gibson The Senses Considered As Perceptual Systems , 1967 .

[28]  Edouard Gentaz,et al.  Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  Donald G. McLaren,et al.  Visual cortex activation in late-onset, Braille naive blind individuals: An fMRI study during semantic and phonological tasks with heard words , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[30]  Kenneth O. Johnson,et al.  The roles and functions of cutaneous mechanoreceptors , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[31]  M. Heller Texture perception in sighted and blind observers , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[32]  F. Rösler,et al.  Auditory memory in congenitally blind adults: a behavioral-electrophysiological investigation. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[33]  Renana H. Ofan,et al.  Visual cortex activation in bilingual blind individuals during use of native and second language. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.

[34]  K Sathian,et al.  Visual cortical activity during tactile perception in the sighted and the visually deprived. , 2005, Developmental psychobiology.

[35]  M. Paré,et al.  Early-blind human subjects localize sound sources better than sighted subjects , 1998, Nature.

[36]  R. Malach,et al.  Early ‘visual’ cortex activation correlates with superior verbal memory performance in the blind , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Cross-modal reorganization of human cortical functions , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[38]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  Haptic study of three-dimensional objects activates extrastriate visual areas , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  E. R Gizewski,et al.  Cross-modal plasticity for sensory and motor activation patterns in blind subjects , 2003, NeuroImage.