Differential recruitment of executive resources during mind wandering

Recent research has shown that mind wandering recruits executive resources away from the external task towards inner thoughts. No studies however have determined whether executive functions are drawn away in a unitary manner during mind wandering episodes, or whether there is variation in specific functions impacted. Accordingly, we examined whether mind wandering differentially modulates three core executive functions-response inhibition, updating of working memory, and mental set shifting. In three experiments, participants performed one of these three executive function tasks and reported their attentional state as either on-task or mind wandering at random intervals. We found that mind wandering led to poorer performance in the response inhibition and working memory tasks, but not the set-shifting task. These findings suggest that mind wandering does not recruit executive functions in a monolithic manner. Rather, it appears to selectively engage certain executive functions, which may reflect the adaptive maintenance of ongoing task performance.

[1]  T. Goldberg,et al.  Brain regions underlying response inhibition and interference monitoring and suppression , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[2]  Dawa T. Phillips,et al.  Mindfulness Training Improves Working Memory Capacity and GRE Performance While Reducing Mind Wandering , 2013, Psychological science.

[3]  J. Alvarez,et al.  Executive Function and the Frontal Lobes: A Meta-Analytic Review , 2006, Neuropsychology Review.

[4]  P. Silvia,et al.  For Whom the Mind Wanders, and When , 2007, Psychological science.

[5]  D. Meyer,et al.  Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Uncovering the Neural Signature of Lapsing Attention: Electrophysiological Signals Predict Errors up to 20 s before They Occur , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  A. Aron The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[8]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources , 1995, Memory & cognition.

[9]  G. Logan Spatial attention and the apprehension of spatial relations. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  The Persistence of Thought , 2012, Psychological science.

[11]  Robert J. Zatorre,et al.  Mental Concerts: Musical Imagery and Auditory Cortex , 2005, Neuron.

[12]  W. Kirchner Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.

[13]  D. Chalmers Consciousness and Cognition , 1990 .

[14]  F. Castellanos,et al.  Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: A neurobiological hypothesis , 2007, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[15]  V. Bruce Unsolved mysteries of the mind : tutorial essays in cognition , 1998 .

[16]  Dylan M. Jones,et al.  Memory updating in working memory : the role of the central executive , 1990 .

[17]  Claire Braboszcz,et al.  Lost in thoughts: Neural markers of low alertness during mind wandering , 2011, NeuroImage.

[18]  S. Monsell Control of mental processes , 2021, Unsolved Mysteries of the Mind.

[19]  M. Schmitter-Edgecombe,et al.  Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks following severe closed-head injury. , 2006, Neuropsychology.

[20]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory , 1995, Nature.

[21]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[22]  J. Smallwood Distinguishing how from why the mind wanders: a process-occurrence framework for self-generated mental activity. , 2013, Psychological bulletin.

[23]  Erik D. Reichle,et al.  Meta-awareness, perceptual decoupling and the wandering mind , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  Todd C. Handy,et al.  Mind wandering and motor control: off-task thinking disrupts the online adjustment of behavior , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[25]  R. Engle Working Memory Capacity as Executive Attention , 2002 .

[26]  Lawrence M. Ward,et al.  Human Neuroscience Original Research Article Differential Synchronization in Default and Task-specific Networks of the Human Brain , 2022 .

[27]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Neural Systems Shared by Visual Imagery and Visual Perception: A Positron Emission Tomography Study , 1997, NeuroImage.

[28]  L. Jarvik,et al.  Stroop color-word test, non-psychotic organic brain syndrome, and chromosome loss in aged twins. , 1971, Journal of gerontology.

[29]  B McElree,et al.  Working memory and focal attention. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[30]  Arthur P. Shimamura,et al.  The role of the prefrontal cortex in dynamic filtering , 2000, Psychobiology.

[31]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[32]  Jelmer P. Borst,et al.  Stroop and picture—word interference are two sides of the same coin , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[33]  K. Christoff,et al.  Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Response to Comment on "Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought" , 2007, Science.

[35]  T. Shallice,et al.  Task Switching : A PDP Model , 2001 .

[36]  S. Carlson,et al.  Distribution of cortical activation during visuospatial n-back tasks as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[37]  John Polich,et al.  P300 and response time from a manual Stroop task , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[38]  Colin M. Macleod Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[39]  Jonathan S. A. Carriere,et al.  Absent-mindedness: Lapses of conscious awareness and everyday cognitive failures , 2006, Consciousness and Cognition.

[40]  K. Christoff,et al.  Undirected thought: Neural determinants and correlates , 2012, Brain Research.

[41]  Colin M. Macleod,et al.  Interdimensional interference in the Stroop effect: uncovering the cognitive and neural anatomy of attention , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  Dwight J. Kravitz,et al.  Attentional control: Temporal relationships within the fronto-parietal network , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : General The Role of Mind-Wandering in Measurements of General Aptitude , 2012 .

[44]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Working Memory: A View from Neuroimaging , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[45]  A. Jensen,et al.  The Stroop color-word test: a review. , 1966, Acta psychologica.

[46]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The lights are on but no one’s home: Meta-awareness and the decoupling of attention when the mind wanders , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[47]  Kevin S. Brown,et al.  Cooperation between the default mode network and the frontal–parietal network in the production of an internal train of thought , 2012, Brain Research.

[48]  M. Kane,et al.  Conducting the train of thought: working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[49]  Jonathan W. Schooler,et al.  Re-representing consciousness: dissociations between experience and meta-consciousness , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  Jonathan Smallwood,et al.  Going AWOL in the Brain: Mind Wandering Reduces Cortical Analysis of External Events , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  The role of prefrontal regions in the Stroop task , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  D. Stuss,et al.  Stroop performance in focal lesion patients: dissociation of processes and frontal lobe lesion location , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  L. Vaughan,et al.  Executive function in daily life: Age-related influences of executive processes on instrumental activities of daily living. , 2010, Psychology and aging.

[54]  J. Smallwood,et al.  The restless mind. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.

[55]  Steve Majerus,et al.  Neural Correlates of Ongoing Conscious Experience: Both Task-Unrelatedness and Stimulus-Independence Are Related to Default Network Activity , 2011, PloS one.

[56]  Stewart H. Mostofsky,et al.  Response Inhibition and Response Selection: Two Sides of the Same Coin , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[57]  Kevin Fitzpatrick,et al.  Slow Fluctuations in Attentional Control of Sensory Cortex , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[58]  M. Kane,et al.  What Mind Wandering Reveals About Executive-Control Abilities and Failures , 2012 .

[59]  S. Monsell Task switching , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[60]  Todd C. Handy,et al.  Mind Wandering and the Adaptive Control of Attentional Resources , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  M. D’Esposito Working memory. , 2008, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[62]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition , 2010, NeuroImage.

[63]  J. Stroop Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. , 1992 .