Reaching beyond spatial perception: Effects of intended future actions on visually guided prehension
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] P. Fitts. The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[2] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[3] D. Chaffin,et al. An investigation of fitts' law using a wide range of movement amplitudes. , 1976, Journal of motor behavior.
[4] S. T. Klapp,et al. Motor programming within a sequence of responses. , 1976, Journal of motor behavior.
[5] S T Klapp,et al. Programmed control of aimed movements revisited the role of target visibility and symmetry. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] Michael A. Arbib,et al. Perceptual Structures and Distributed Motor Control , 1981 .
[7] Donald A. Norman,et al. Simulating a Skilled Typist: A Study of Skilled Cognitive-Motor Performance , 1982, Cogn. Sci..
[8] E. R. Crossman,et al. Feedback Control of Hand-Movement and Fitts' Law , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[9] Gerard P. van Galen,et al. Structural complexity of motor patterns: A study on reaction times and movement times of handwritten letters , 1984 .
[10] K. J. Cole,et al. Control of multimovement coordination: sensorimotor mechanisms in speech motor programming. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.
[11] T. Flash,et al. The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[12] C. Atkeson,et al. Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[13] K. J. Cole,et al. Coordination of three-joint digit movements for rapid finger-thumb grasp. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.
[14] M. Jeannerod,et al. Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.
[15] Gerard P. van Galen,et al. Levels of Motor Programming in Writing Familiar and Unfamiliar Symbols , 1988 .
[16] S. Lehman. The Neural and Behavioural Organization of Goal‐Directed Movements , 1990, Neurology.
[17] D. Rosenbaum,et al. Planning macroscopic aspects of manual control , 1992 .
[18] D. Rosenbaum,et al. Time course of movement planning: selection of handgrips for object manipulation. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[19] Umberto Castiello,et al. Insights into the reach to grasp movement , 1994 .
[20] M. Jeannerod. The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[21] Hiroshi Sekiya,et al. Movement variability as a function of accuracy demand in programmed serial aiming responses , 1995 .
[22] M. Gentilucci,et al. Planning for action , 2020, Coaching with Research in Mind.
[23] M. E. McCarty,et al. Problem solving in infancy: the emergence of an action plan. , 1999, Developmental psychology.
[24] M. Mon-Williams,et al. A test between two hypotheses and a possible third way for the control of prehension , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[25] S. Iversen. Motor control , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[26] M. E. McCarty,et al. The Beginnings of Tool Use by Infants and Toddlers , 2001 .
[27] C. MacKenzie,et al. The speed-accuracy trade-off in manual prehension: effects of movement amplitude, object size and object width on kinematic characteristics , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[28] J. F. Soechting,et al. Effect of target size on spatial and temporal characteristics of a pointing movement in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.