Self-operated stimuli improve subsequent visual motion processing
暂无分享,去创建一个
David J. Ostry | Silvio P. Sabatini | Vittorio Sanguineti | Giulia Sedda | D. Ostry | V. Sanguineti | S. Sabatini | G. Sedda
[1] N. Prins. The psi-marginal adaptive method: How to give nuisance parameters the attention they deserve (no more, no less). , 2013, Journal of vision.
[2] Melvyn A Goodale,et al. Motor Force Field Learning Influences Visual Processing of Target Motion , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[3] Duje Tadin,et al. Disentangling locus of perceptual learning in the visual hierarchy of motion processing , 2018, Scientific Reports.
[4] D. Henriques,et al. Sensory recalibration of hand position following visuomotor adaptation. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[5] E. Adelson,et al. Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.
[6] Jonathan W. Pillow,et al. Single-trial spike trains in parietal cortex reveal discrete steps during decision-making , 2015, Science.
[7] J. B. Mulligan,et al. Effect of contrast on the perceived direction of a moving plaid , 1990, Vision Research.
[8] M. Carandini,et al. Testing the Bayesian model of perceived speed , 2002, Vision Research.
[9] Dale Purves,et al. An empirical explanation of aperture effects , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[10] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[11] T. Albright,et al. Motion coherency rules are form-cue invariant , 1992, Vision Research.
[12] H. Wilson,et al. Perceived direction of moving two-dimensional patterns , 1990, Vision Research.
[13] W. Prinz,et al. Seeing while moving: Measuring the online influence of action on perception , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[14] Hans Wallach. Über visuell wahrgenommene Bewegungsrichtung , 1935 .
[15] J. Krakauer,et al. Human sensorimotor learning: adaptation, skill, and beyond , 2011, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[16] Denis G. Pelli,et al. ECVP '07 Abstracts , 2007, Perception.
[17] Nava Rubin,et al. The oblique plaid effect , 2004, Vision Research.
[18] A. Wohlschläger,et al. Visual motion priming by invisible actions , 2000, Vision Research.
[19] Sethu Vijayakumar,et al. Unifying the Sensory and Motor Components of Sensorimotor Adaptation , 2008, NIPS.
[20] Marianna Semprini,et al. Consolidation of human somatosensory memory during motor learning , 2018, Behavioural Brain Research.
[21] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[22] Claude L. Fennema,et al. Velocity determination in scenes containing several moving objects , 1979 .
[23] C. S. Harris. Adaptation to Displaced Vision: Visual, Motor, or Proprioceptive Change? , 1963, Science.
[24] Rebecca A. Champion,et al. Perceived direction of plaid motion is not predicted by component speeds , 2007, Vision Research.
[25] R. Held,et al. MOVEMENT-PRODUCED STIMULATION IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF VISUALLY GUIDED BEHAVIOR. , 1963, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[26] W. Einhäuser,et al. Cognition modulates action-to-perception transfer in ambiguous perception. , 2018, Journal of vision.
[27] Jeremy D Wong,et al. Somatosensory Plasticity and Motor Learning , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[28] Iseult A. M. Beets,et al. Nonvisual motor learning improves visual motion perception: evidence from violating the two-thirds power law. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.
[29] Dennis M. Levi. A new Editor-in-Chief for Journal of Vision , 2013 .
[30] P. A. Beckett. Development of the third component in prism adaptation: effects of active and passive movement. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[31] Jeounghoon Kim,et al. Dependence of plaid motion coherence on component grating directions , 1993, Vision Research.
[32] C. Tyler,et al. Bayesian adaptive estimation of psychometric slope and threshold , 1999, Vision Research.
[33] W. Prinz,et al. Perceptual resonance: action-induced modulation of perception , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[34] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. Optimal inference explains the perceptual coherence of visual motion stimuli. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[35] Zijiang J. He,et al. Seeing grating-textured surface begins at the border. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[36] T. D. Albright,et al. Transparency and coherence in human motion perception , 1990, Nature.
[37] A. Wohlschläger. Mental object rotation and the planning of hand movements , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[38] T D Albright,et al. Visual motion perception. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[39] Edward H. Adelson,et al. Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[40] Hannah J. Block,et al. Somatosensory changes associated with motor skill learning. , 2020, Journal of neurophysiology.
[41] Andrew A G Mattar,et al. Motor learning and its sensory effects: time course of perceptual change and its presence with gradual introduction of load. , 2013, Journal of neurophysiology.
[42] T. Milner,et al. Functionally Specific Changes in Resting-State Sensorimotor Networks after Motor Learning , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[43] Fulvio Domini,et al. Visuomotor Adaptation Changes Stereoscopic Depth Perception and Tactile Discrimination , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[44] C. Law,et al. Neural correlates of perceptual learning in a sensory-motor, but not a sensory, cortical area , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.
[45] D R Badcock,et al. Motion coherence across different chromatic axes. , 1996, Vision research.
[46] D. Purves,et al. How biological vision succeeds in the physical world , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.