Time course of cross-hemispheric spatial updating in the human parietal cortex

In human parietal cortex, the retinal location of a just seen visual stimulus is updated from one hemisphere to the other, when a horizontal eye movement brings the representation of the stimulus into the opposite visual hemifield. The present study aimed to elucidate the time course of this process. Twelve subjects performed an updating task, in which a filled circle was shown before a horizontal saccade, requiring updating of stimulus location, and a control task without visual stimulation before the saccade. Electroencephalogram (EEG) and electrooculogram (EOG) were recorded while subjects performed the tasks and LORETA source analysis was performed on event-related potential (ERP) components. ERP amplitudes were more positive in the updating condition in comparison to the control condition in two latency windows. An early positive wave starting at about 50 ms after saccade offset and originating in the posterior parietal cortex contralateral to saccade direction probably reflects the integration of saccade-related and visual information and thus the updating process. A shift of the representation of the to-be-updated stimulus to the opposite hemisphere is reflected in a later component starting approximately 400 ms after saccade offset, which is related to memory and originates in the PPC ipsilateral to saccade direction and thus contralateral to the spatial location of the updated visual stimulus.

[1]  W Skrandies,et al.  Topography of visually evoked brain activity during eye movements: lambda waves, saccadic suppression, and discrimination performance. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[2]  Christian Bellebaum,et al.  Post-saccadic updating of visual space in the posterior parietal cortex in humans , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.

[3]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  C. Colby,et al.  Spatial representations for action in parietal cortex. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[5]  R. Andersen,et al.  Saccade-related activity in the lateral intraparietal area. II. Spatial properties. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  M. Mintun,et al.  Positron emission tomography study of voluntary saccadic eye movements and spatial working memory. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[8]  C. Bruce,et al.  Primate frontal eye fields. III. Maintenance of a spatially accurate saccade signal. , 1990, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  W. Heide,et al.  Cortical control of double‐step saccades: Implications for spatial orientation , 1995, Annals of neurology.

[10]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Saccadic dysmetria in a patient with a right frontoparietal lesion. The importance of corollary discharge for accurate spatial behaviour. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  R. J. Seitz,et al.  Activation of frontoparietal cortices during memorized triple‐step sequences of saccadic eye movements: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[12]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Saccadic eye movements towards stimuli triggered by prior saccades , 1976, Vision Research.

[13]  R. Kleiser,et al.  Neural correlates of reafference: evoked brain activity during motion perception and saccadic eye movements , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  L E Mays,et al.  Saccades are spatially, not retinocentrically, coded. , 1980, Science.

[15]  B. Gaymard,et al.  The frontal eye field is involved in spatial short-term memory but not in reflexive saccade inhibition , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The time course of perisaccadic receptive field shifts in the lateral intraparietal area of the monkey. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  M. Sereno,et al.  Mapping of Contralateral Space in Retinotopic Coordinates by a Parietal Cortical Area in Humans , 2001, Science.

[18]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Saccadic eye movements to flashed targets , 1976, Vision Research.

[19]  R. Sperry Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[20]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Spatial processing in the monkey frontal eye field. I. Predictive visual responses. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  D. Lehmann,et al.  Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: a new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[22]  T. Vilis,et al.  Remapping the remembered target location for anti-saccades in human posterior parietal cortex. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[24]  R. Wurtz,et al.  A Pathway in Primate Brain for Internal Monitoring of Movements , 2002, Science.

[25]  B. Bridgeman A review of the role of efference copy in sensory and oculomotor control systems , 1995, Annals of Biomedical Engineering.

[26]  T. Vilis,et al.  Gaze-Centered Updating of Visual Space in Human Parietal Cortex , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  R M Müri,et al.  Effects of single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation over the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices during memory-guided saccades in humans. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  R. Andersen,et al.  Coding of intention in the posterior parietal cortex , 1997, Nature.

[29]  J L Lancaster,et al.  Automated Talairach Atlas labels for functional brain mapping , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[30]  C. Genovese,et al.  Spatial Updating in Human Parietal Cortex , 2003, Neuron.

[31]  R. Kleiser,et al.  Electrophysiological correlates of human intrasaccadic processing , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  W. Heide,et al.  Combined deficits of saccades and visuo-spatial orientation after cortical lesions , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[34]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[35]  R. Andersen,et al.  Motor intention activity in the macaque's lateral intraparietal area. II. Changes of motor plan. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  G. Thickbroom,et al.  Cerebral events preceding self-paced and visually triggered saccades. A study of presaccadic potentials. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[37]  R. Andersen,et al.  Memory related motor planning activity in posterior parietal cortex of macaque , 1988, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  I Daum,et al.  The role of the human thalamus in processing corollary discharge. , 2005, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[40]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[41]  Paul J. Laurienti,et al.  An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets , 2003, NeuroImage.

[42]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Word and Nonword Repetition Within- and Across-Modality: An Event-Related Potential Study , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[43]  R. Andersen,et al.  Motor intention activity in the macaque's lateral intraparietal area. I. Dissociation of motor plan from sensory memory. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  J W Belliveau,et al.  Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1995, Science.