Deconstructing Hunting Returns: Can We Reconstruct and Predict Payoffs from Pursuing Prey?

Explaining variation in hunter-gatherer livelihoods hinges on our ability to predict the tradeoffs and opportunities of pursuing different kinds of prey. Central to this problem is the commonly held assumption that larger animals provide higher returns upon encounter than smaller ones. However, to test this assumption, actualistic observations of hunting payoffs must be comparable across different social, technological, and ecological contexts. In this meta-analysis, we revisit published and unpublished estimates of prey return rates (n = 217 from 181 prey types) to assess, first, whether they are methodologically comparable, and second, whether they correlate with body size. We find systematic inter-study differences in how carcass yield, energetic content, and foraging returns are calculated. We correct for these inconsistencies first by calculating new estimates of energetic yield (kcals per kg live weight) and processing costs for over 300 species of terrestrial and avian game. We then recalculate on-encounter returns using a standardized formula. We find that body size is a poor predictor of on-encounter return rate, while prey characteristics and behavior, mode of procurement, and hunting technology are better predictors. Although prey body size correlates well with processing costs and edibility, relationships with pursuit time and energetic value per kilogram are relatively weak.

[1]  R. Bliege Bird,et al.  Why Women Hunt , 2008, Current Anthropology.

[2]  R. Bird,et al.  Interpreting abundance indices: some zooarchaeological implications of Martu foraging , 2010 .

[3]  L. Henrikson Frozen bison and fur trapper's journals: building a prey choice model for Idaho's Snake River Plain , 2004 .

[4]  Graham H. Pyke,et al.  Optimal Foraging: A Selective Review of Theory and Tests , 1977, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[5]  R. Bird,et al.  Martu Children’s Hunting Strategies in the Western Desert, Australia , 2017 .

[6]  D. Macdonald,et al.  A socio-economic survey of pangolin hunting in Assam, Northeast India , 2018, Nature Conservation.

[7]  J. Jones,et al.  To kill a kangaroo: understanding the decision to pursue high-risk/high-gain resources , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  Todd A. Surovell,et al.  Paleolithic population growth pulses evidenced by small animal exploitation , 1999, Science.

[9]  T. Schoener A Brief History of Optimal Foraging Ecology , 1987 .

[10]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Hunting income patterns among the Hadza: big game, common goods, foraging goals and the evolution of the human diet. , 1991, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[11]  R. Bliege Bird,et al.  Risky Pursuits: Martu Hunting and the Effects of Prey Mobility: Reply to Ugan and Simms , 2012, American Antiquity.

[12]  R. Bettinger,et al.  Central Place Models of Acorn and Mussel Processing , 1997 .

[13]  F. Bayham Factors Influencing the Archaic Pattern of Animal Exploitation , 1979 .

[14]  James A. Yost,et al.  Shotguns, Blowguns, and Spears: The Analysis of Technological Efficiency , 1983 .

[15]  Douglas Bird,et al.  The hunting handicap: costly signaling in human foraging strategies , 2001, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[16]  Yukio Kuchikura,et al.  Efficiency and focus of blowpipe hunting among Semaq Beri Hunter-Gatherers of Peninsular Malaysia , 1988 .

[17]  Kim Hill,et al.  A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity , 2000 .

[18]  Mark S. Boyce,et al.  Seasonality, Fasting Endurance, and Body Size in Mammals , 1985, The American Naturalist.

[19]  Bruce Winterhalder,et al.  Foraging strategies in the boreal forest: An analysis of Cree hunting and gathering , 1981 .

[20]  K. Hill,et al.  Hunting and human evolution , 1982 .

[21]  Jerry D. Moore Behavioral Ecology and Hunter. Gatherer Foraging: An Example from the Great Basin , 1991, American Antiquity.

[22]  D. Madsen,et al.  Size matters only sometimes: the energy-risk trade-offs of Holocene prey acquisition in the Bonneville basin, western USA , 2020, Archaeological and Anthropological Sciences.

[23]  K. Hawkes,et al.  why hunters gather: optimal foraging and the Aché of eastern Paraguay , 1982 .

[24]  R. Bird,et al.  Seasonality in Primates: Human hunting seasonality , 2005 .

[25]  Andrew Sih,et al.  Optimal diet theory: when does it work, and when and why does it fail? , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[26]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Showing off: Tests of an hypothesis about men's foraging goals , 1991 .

[27]  Douglas W. Yu,et al.  Modelling the long-term sustainability of indigenous hunting in Manu National Park, Peru: Landscape-scale management implications for Amazonia , 2009 .

[28]  LouisLiebenberg,et al.  Persistence Hunting by Modern Hunter- Gatherers , 2006 .

[29]  K. Hawkes Ethnoarchaeology and Plio-Pleistocene sites: Some lessons from the Hadza , 2016 .

[30]  D. Schmitt,et al.  Small prey hunting technology and zooarchaeological measures of taxonomic diversity and abundance: Ethnoarchaeological evidence from Central African forest foragers , 2005 .

[31]  A. Sih Optimal Diet: The Relative Importance of the Parameters , 1979, The American Naturalist.

[32]  L. Liebenberg Persistence Hunting by Modern Hunter‐Gatherers , 2006, Current Anthropology.

[33]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Male strategies and Plio-Pleistocene archaeology. , 2002, Journal of human evolution.

[34]  B. Winterhalder Boreal Foraging Strategies , 1983 .

[35]  M. Janssen,et al.  Benefits of Grouping and Cooperative Hunting Among Ache Hunter–Gatherers: Insights from an Agent-Based Foraging Model , 2014 .

[36]  C. Pond An evolutionary and functional view of mammalian adipose tissue , 1992, Proceedings of the Nutrition Society.

[37]  D. Schmitt,et al.  Upper Paleolithic Net-Hunting, Small Prey Exploitation, and Women's Work Effort: A View from the Ethnographic and Ethnoarchaeological Record of the Congo Basin , 2002 .

[38]  Duncan Metcalfe,et al.  A Model for Exploring the Optimal Trade‐off between Field Processing and Transport , 1992 .

[39]  S. Simms Behavioral Ecology and Hunter-Gatherer Foraging: An example from the Great Basin: An example from the Great Basin , 1987 .

[40]  D. Wilkie,et al.  Why Do Mbuti Hunters Use Nets? Ungulate Hunting Efficiency of Archers and Net‐Hunters in the Ituri Rain Forest , 1991 .

[41]  L. Satterthwait Socioeconomic Implications of Australian Aboriginal Net Hunting , 1987 .

[42]  S. Simms,et al.  On Prey Mobility, Prey Rank, and Foraging Goals , 2012, American Antiquity.

[43]  M. Alvard Shotguns and sustainable hunting in the Neotropics , 1995, Oryx.

[44]  M. Baker 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibility , 2016, Nature.

[45]  T. E. White A Method of Calculating the Dietary Percentage of Various Food Animals Utilized by Aboriginal Peoples , 1953, American Antiquity.

[46]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Hadza Scavenging: Implications for Plio/Pleistocene Hominid Subsistence , 1988, Current Anthropology.

[47]  Y. Kuchikura Fishing in the Tropical Rain Forest : Utilization of Aquatic Resources among the Semaq Beri Hunter-Gatherers of Peninsular Malaysia , 1996 .

[48]  H. Kaplan,et al.  Procurement Technology and Prey Mortality Among Indigenous Neotropical Hunters , 2019, Human Predators and Prey Mortality.

[49]  N. Jones A selfish origin for human food sharing: Tolerated theft , 1984 .

[50]  Y. Kuchikura Subsistence ecology among Semaq Beri hunter-gatherers of Peninsular Malaysia , 1987 .

[51]  J. Broughton Late Holocene resource intensification in the Sacramento Valley , 1994 .

[52]  J. Yravedra,et al.  On meat eating and human evolution: A taphonomic analysis of BK4b (Upper Bed II, Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania), and its bearing on hominin megafaunal consumption , 2014 .

[53]  J. Speth Early hominid hunting and scavenging: the role of meat as an energy source , 1989 .

[54]  David B. Madsen,et al.  Mass collecting and the diet breadth model: A Great Basin example , 1998 .

[55]  J. Broughton,et al.  Prey Body Size and Ranking in Zooarchaeology: Theory, Empirical Evidence, and Applications from the Northern Great Basin , 2011, American Antiquity.

[56]  Bruce Winterhalder,et al.  Hunter-gatherer foraging strategies : ethnographic and archeological analyses , 1983 .

[57]  Jeremy Koster,et al.  Hunting with Dogs in Nicaragua: An Optimal Foraging Approach , 2008, Current Anthropology.

[58]  J. O'connell,et al.  Analysis of Kangaroo Body Part Transport Among the Alyawara of Central Australia , 1989 .

[59]  Robert H. Lowie,et al.  Minor ceremonies of the Crow Indians , 1924 .

[60]  E. Jones Prey choice, mass collecting, and the wild European rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus) , 2006 .

[61]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Foraging decisions among Aché hunter-gatherers: New data and implications for optimal foraging models☆ , 1987 .

[62]  C. Fowler,et al.  Willard Z. Park's ethnograhic notes on the Northern Paiute of western Nevada, 1933-1940 , 1991 .

[63]  A. Sih Optimal Foraging: Partial Consumption of Prey , 1980, The American Naturalist.

[64]  E. Morin,et al.  Presumed Symbolic Use of Diurnal Raptors by Neanderthals , 2012, PloS one.

[65]  Raymond Hames,et al.  A comparison of the efficiencies of the shotgun and the bow in neotropical forest hunting , 1979 .

[66]  H. Hara Hare Indians and their world , 1980 .

[67]  A. Noss The Economic Importance of Communal Net Hunting Among the BaAka of the Central African Republic , 1997 .

[68]  T. Schoener The compression hypothesis and temporal resource partitioning. , 1974, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[69]  R. Bird,et al.  Contemporary Shellfish Gathering Strategies among the Meriam of the Torres Strait Islands, Australia: Testing Predictions of a Central Place Foraging Model , 1997 .

[70]  A. Ugan Does Size Matter? Body Size, Mass Collecting, and Their Implications for Understanding Prehistoric Foraging Behavior , 2005, American Antiquity.

[71]  James H. Brown,et al.  The origin of allometric scaling laws in biology from genomes to ecosystems: towards a quantitative unifying theory of biological structure and organization , 2005, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[72]  T. Tanno The Mbuti net-hunters in the Ituri Forest, Eastern Zaire - their hunting activities and band composition , 1976 .

[73]  R. Barkai,et al.  The importance of large prey animals during the Pleistocene and the implications of their extinction on the use of dietary ethnographic analogies , 2020 .

[74]  K. Schmidt-Nielsen,et al.  Scaling, why is animal size so important? , 1984 .

[75]  R. Bliege Bird,et al.  The social significance of subtle signals , 2018, Nature Human Behaviour.

[76]  S. Washburn,et al.  The Evolution of Hunting , 1968 .

[77]  Henry D. Prange,et al.  Scaling of Skeletal Mass to Body Mass in Birds and Mammals , 1979, The American Naturalist.

[78]  K. Hill,et al.  Macronutrient modifications of optimal foraging theory: An approach using indifference curves applied to some modern foragers , 1988 .

[79]  R. Bliege Bird,et al.  Mass procurement and prey rankings: insights from the European rabbit , 2020, Archaeological and Anthropological Sciences.

[80]  M. Alvard Testing the “ecologically noble savage” hypothesis: Interspecific prey choice by Piro hunters of Amazonian Peru , 1993 .

[81]  R. Bird,et al.  Megafauna in a continent of small game: Archaeological implications of Martu Camel hunting in Australia's Western Desert , 2013 .

[82]  H. Bunn,et al.  Variability in Bone Assemblage Formation from Hadza Hunting, Scavenging, and Carcass Processing , 1988 .

[83]  David R. Anderson,et al.  Model selection and multimodel inference : a practical information-theoretic approach , 2003 .

[84]  I. Hershkovitz,et al.  Man the Fat Hunter: The Demise of Homo erectus and the Emergence of a New Hominin Lineage in the Middle Pleistocene (ca. 400 kyr) Levant , 2011, PloS one.

[85]  George B. Silberbauer,et al.  Hunter and habitat in the central Kalahari Desert , 1983 .

[86]  R. Bird,et al.  In Pursuit of Mobile Prey: Martu Hunting Strategies and Archaeofaunal Interpretation , 2009, American Antiquity.

[87]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Neotropical Hunting among the Aché of Eastern Paraguay , 1983 .

[88]  R. BliegeBird,et al.  Signaling Theory, Strategic Interaction, and Symbolic Capital1 , 2005, Current Anthropology.

[89]  M. Symonds,et al.  A brief guide to model selection, multimodel inference and model averaging in behavioural ecology using Akaike’s information criterion , 2010, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[90]  D. Schmitt,et al.  When bigger is not better: The economics of hunting megafauna and its implications for Plio-Pleistocene hunter-gatherers , 2016 .

[91]  R. Macarthur,et al.  On Optimal Use of a Patchy Environment , 1966, The American Naturalist.

[92]  G. Silberbauer 12. Hunter/Gatherers of the Central Kalahari , 1981 .

[93]  J. Speth The Paleoanthropology and Archaeology of Big-Game Hunting , 2010 .

[94]  C. Shaffer,et al.  Sustainability and comanagement of subsistence hunting in an indigenous reserve in Guyana , 2017, Conservation biology : the journal of the Society for Conservation Biology.

[95]  R. V. Emmons Jamaican Site Designation—A Critique , 1953, American Antiquity.

[96]  K. Hill,et al.  The Evolutionary Ecology of Food Acquisition , 2017 .

[97]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Hunter-gatherer studies and human evolution: A very selective review. , 2018, American journal of physical anthropology.

[98]  Kim Hill,et al.  Seasonal variance in the diet of Ache hunter-gatherers in Eastern Paraguay , 1984 .

[99]  K. Hawkes,et al.  Food Choice and Foraging Sites among the Alyawara , 1984, Journal of Anthropological Research.