On the word-superiority effect

SummaryFour experiments tested Johnston's (1981) hierarchical-activation account and McClelland and Rumelhart's (1981) interactive-activation account of the word-superiority effect. The hierarchical-activation account claims that the effect depends on masking. Using both a luminance manipulation and direct feature removal, however, we showed that the word-superiority effect can be obtained without a mask. Moreover, when words were made hard to read by displaying them in a column, the context advantage with both the luminance and the feature-removal techniques was reduced to zero. With masking, in contrast, a context effect remained because of lower accuracy in the no-context (control) condition. Thus, masking confounds enhancement on context trials with inhibition on no-context trials and is neither a necessary nor a desirable procedure with which to demonstrate a context advantage.The advantage of word context is reduced when words are printed with extra space between the letters. The amount of space needed depends on the size of the letters — the reduction requires a manipulation about the size of a blank character. Although both define visual features spatially, neither model includes an internal representation of space. Thus, neither model can represent the configural manipulations, and neither can explain why column orientation and blank characters reduce the word-superiority effect.

[1]  H. Bouma,et al.  Visual search and reading : eye movements and functional visual field : a tutorial review , 1978 .

[2]  J. L. Mcclelland Preliminary letter identification in the perception of words and nonwords. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[3]  C W Eriksen,et al.  The use of a visual mask may seriously confound your experiment , 1980, Perception & psychophysics.

[4]  Jamie I. D. Campbell,et al.  Identification, localization, and “iconic memory”: An evaluation of the bar-probe task , 1981, Memory & cognition.

[5]  Allen R. Hanson,et al.  Context in word recognition , 1976, Pattern Recognition.

[6]  Douglas J. K. Mewhort,et al.  The rate of word integration and the overprinting paradigm , 1980 .

[7]  G. M. Reicher Perceptual recognition as a function of meaninfulness of stimulus material. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  D J Mewhort,et al.  On familiarity effects in visual information processing. , 1980, Canadian journal of psychology.

[9]  Mewhort Dj Accuracy and order of report in tachistoscopic identification. , 1974 .

[10]  D. Mewhort,et al.  Mechanisms of word identification. , 1977 .

[11]  H. Bouma Visual interference in the parafoveal recognition of initial and final letters of words. , 1973, Vision research.

[12]  Marilyn Jager Adams,et al.  Models of word recognition , 1979, Cognitive Psychology.

[13]  S. Jay Samuels,et al.  The effects of letter degradation and letter spacing on word recognition , 1976 .

[14]  W. Estes The Locus of Inferential and Perceptual Processes in Letter Identification. , 1975 .

[15]  D. Massaro,et al.  The role of lateral masking and orthographic structure in letter and word recognition. , 1979, Acta psychologica.

[16]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: part 1.: an account of basic findings , 1988 .

[17]  James C Johnston,et al.  A test of the Sophisticated Guessing Theory of word perception , 1978, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  Daniel Holender,et al.  Disruptive effect of precueing on the identification of letters in masked words: an attentional interpretation , 1985 .

[19]  Sandra L. Newsome,et al.  Do small visual angles produce a word superiority effect or differential lateral masking? , 1980 .

[20]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[21]  D J Mewhort,et al.  Blank characters in tachistoscopic recognition: space has both a symbolic and a sensory role. , 1982, Canadian journal of psychology.

[22]  J. Bruner,et al.  Familiarity of Letter Sequences and Tachistoscopic Identification , 1954 .

[23]  D J Mewhort,et al.  Spatial localization in sequential letter displays. , 1975, Canadian journal of psychology.

[24]  D. J. K. Mewhort,et al.  Scanning and the Distribution of Attention: The Current Status of Heron’s Sensory-Motor Theory , 1984 .

[25]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. , 1982, Psychological review.

[26]  Keith E. Stanovich,et al.  Some Boundary Conditions for a Word Superiority Effect* , 1982 .

[27]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Experimental tests of a hierarchical model of word identification , 1980 .

[28]  D. Holender Identification of letters in words and of single letters with pre- and postknowledge vs. postknowledge of the alternatives , 1979 .

[29]  M. P. Bryden,et al.  Left-right differences in tachistoscopic recognition as a function of familiarity and pattern orientation. , 1970 .

[30]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Visual factors in word perception , 1973 .

[31]  James L. McClelland Putting Knowledge in its Place: A Scheme for Programming Parallel Processing Structures on the Fly , 1988, Cogn. Sci..