Step-Wise Evolution of Mental Models of Electric C ircuits: A "Learning-Aloud" Case Study

Various methods have been tried for fostering conceptual change in science including the use of analogies, discrepant events, and visual models. In this article we describe an approach to teaching complex models in science that takes a model construction cycle of generation, evaluation, and modification as an organizing framework for thinking about when to use each of the previous strategies. This approach of model evolution uses all of the previous methods as students are led to reassess and revise their model many times in the course of the lessons. We reported on the case study of a student in a tutoring experiment using this approach in the study of electric circuits. We concentrated on the student's moments of surprise as motivators of conceptual change. Most of these came from discrepant events, but 1 of them appeared to come from the student's own sensed lack of coherence in an intermediate model. In this case study, the teaching method appears to lead to the construction of an explanatory model that is fairly deeply understood by the student in the sense that it can generate predictions and coherent explanations of a complex system in a transfer problem. Some of our conclusions and hypotheses generated with respect to learning processes are as follows: (a) Discrepant events produced reactions of surprise and were eventually followed by model revisions, leading us to hypothesize a motivating and guiding role for these events; (b) the subject was able to map and apply an air pressure analogy used for electric potential and continued to exhibit traces of it through the posttest interview; (c) the subject's spontaneous use of similar depictive hand motions during the instruction and during the posttest provides initial evidence that the instruction fostered development of dynamic mental models, such as those of fluid-like flows caused by pressure differences, that can generate new mental simulations for understanding relatively difficult transfer problems. This leads us to describe the core of her new knowledge as explanatory models at an intermediate level of generality that allow her to run imagistic simulations and to hypothesize a "transfer of runnability" from the analog conception to the model in this case; (d) we hypothesize that the process underlying model generations and revisions was 1 of scaffolded abductive knowledge construction rather than induction or deduction; that evaluation and revision cycles can make up for the conjectural nature of individual abductions; and that engagement and comprehension in the cycle was fostered by small step sizes for revisions from using multiple "small" discrepant events and analogies built into the lessons.

[1]  P. Thagard,et al.  Abductive Reasoning: Logic, Visual Thinking, and Coherence , 1997 .

[2]  L. Darden Theory Change in Science: Strategies from Mendelian Genetics , 1991 .

[3]  Lorenzo Magnani,et al.  Model-Based Creative Abduction , 1999 .

[4]  John J. Clement,et al.  Use of a computer simulation to develop mental simulations for understanding relative motion concepts , 1999 .

[5]  Marcia C. Linn,et al.  The effect of computer simulations on introductory thermodynamics understanding , 1993 .

[6]  A. Ortony,et al.  Similarity and Analogical Reasoning , 1991 .

[7]  Lorenzo Magnani,et al.  Model-Based Reasoning in Scientific Discovery , 1999, Springer US.

[8]  Robert Fritz,et al.  The Path of Least Resistance , 1986 .

[9]  Melvin S. Steinberg,et al.  Constructive Model Evolution in the Study of Electric Circuits , 1997 .

[10]  J. Roschelle,et al.  Misconceptions Reconceived: A Constructivist Analysis of Knowledge in Transition , 1994 .

[11]  Paul Thagard,et al.  The Best Explanation: Criteria for Theory Choice , 1978 .

[12]  D. Gentner,et al.  Flowing waters or teeming crowds: Mental models of electricity , 1982 .

[13]  David Hammer,et al.  Epistemological Beliefs in Introductory Physics , 1994 .

[14]  J. Clement Analysis of Clinical Interviews: Foundations and model viability , 2000 .

[15]  Rand J. Spiro,et al.  Multiple analogies for complex concepts: antidotes for analogy-induced misconception in advanced knowledge acquisition , 1988 .

[16]  J.-L. Closset,et al.  Le raisonnement séquentiel en électrocinétique , 1983 .

[17]  Amos Dreyfus,et al.  Applying the cognitive conflict strategy for conceptual change - some implications, difficulties, and problems , 1990 .

[18]  L. E. Klopfer,et al.  Cognitive research and the design of science instruction , 1982 .

[19]  Melvin S. Steinberg,et al.  Using models to teach electricity—The CASTLE project , 1993 .

[20]  J. Gregory Trafton,et al.  Effective Tutoring Techniques: A Comparison of Human Tutors and Intelligent Tutoring Systems , 1992 .

[21]  R. Finke,et al.  Principles of mental imagery , 1989 .

[22]  William F. Brewer,et al.  The Role of Anomalous Data in Knowledge Acquisition: A Theoretical Framework and Implications for Science Instruction , 1993 .

[23]  David Topper,et al.  The Essential Tension: Selected Studies in Scientific Tradition and Change , 1979 .

[24]  J. Clement Learning via Model Construction and Criticism , 1989 .

[25]  John J. Clement,et al.  Evolving Mental Models of Electric Circuits , 2001 .

[26]  Walter Connolly Transient lamp lighting with high‐tech capacitors* , 1987 .

[27]  J. Bergeron,et al.  Models of Understanding. , 1983 .

[28]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  Why a Diagram is (Sometimes) Worth Ten Thousand Words , 1987, Cogn. Sci..

[29]  John J. Clement,et al.  Learning via model construction and criticism: protocol evidence on sources of creativity in science , 1988 .

[30]  Joseph D. Novak,et al.  Misconceptions in Science and Mathematics. Proceedings of the International Seminar. (Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA, June 20-22, 1983). , 1983 .

[31]  B. Eylon,et al.  Potential difference and current in simple electric circuits: A study of students’ concepts , 1983 .

[32]  Ronald A. Finke,et al.  Creative Imagery: Discoveries and inventions in Visualization , 1990 .

[33]  Bruce A. Sherwood,et al.  Electric and magnetic interactions , 1994 .

[34]  Barbara Y. White,et al.  Intelligent Tutoring Systems Based Upon Qualitative Model Evolutions , 1986, AAAI.

[35]  D. Gentner,et al.  Cultural models in language and thought: How people construct mental models , 1987 .

[36]  Ruth Stavy,et al.  Using analogy to overcome misconceptions about conservation of matter , 1991 .

[37]  Zoubeida R. Dagher,et al.  Review of studies on the effectiveness of instructional analogies in science education , 1995 .

[38]  J. Clement,et al.  Not all preconceptions are misconceptions: finding ‘anchoring conceptions’ for grounding instruction on students’ intuitions , 1989 .

[39]  Robert Karplus Introductory Physics: A Model Approach , 1969 .

[40]  H A Simon,et al.  The theory of learning by doing. , 1979, Psychological review.

[41]  Fred Goldberg,et al.  Learning Processes in Electric Circuits , 1998 .

[42]  J. Kolodner Educational implications of analogy. A view from case-based reasoning. , 1997, The American psychologist.

[43]  P. Scott,et al.  Constructing Scientific Knowledge in the Classroom , 1994 .

[44]  John R. Frederiksen,et al.  Seeking the causal connection in electricity: shifting among mechanistic perspectives , 1996 .

[45]  J. J. Dupin,et al.  Taking Into Account Student Conceptions in Instructional Strategy: An Example in Physics , 1987 .

[46]  R. Harré The philosophies of science , 1985 .

[47]  R. Mayer Models for Understanding , 1989 .

[48]  Priit Reiska,et al.  Research in science education - past, present, and future , 2001 .

[49]  R. Duit On the role of analogies and metaphors in learning science. , 1991 .

[50]  K. Nelson,et al.  The Conceptual Basis for Language , 1983 .

[51]  Christoph von Rhöneck,et al.  Aspects of understanding electricity : proceedings of an international workshop : an inventory of research results concerning the representation of students' knowledge of electricity and its uses for the improvement of teaching : Ludwigsburg, Sept. 10 to 14, 1984 , 1985 .