When Redundant On-Screen Text in Multimedia Technical Instruction Can Interfere With Learning

It is frequently assumed that presenting the same material in written and spoken form benefits learning and understanding. The present work provides a theoretical justification based on cognitive load theory, and empirical evidence based on controlled experiments, that this assumption can be incorrect. From a theoretical perspective, it is suggested that if learners are required to coordinate and simultaneously process redundant material such as written and spoken text, an excessive working memory load is generated. Three experiments involving a group of 25 technical apprentices compared the effects of simultaneously presenting the same written and auditory textual information as opposed to either temporally separating the two modes or eliminating one of the modes. The first two experiments demonstrated that nonconcurrent presentation of auditory and visual explanations of a diagram proved superior, in terms of ratings of mental load and test scores, to a concurrent presentation of the same explanations when instruction time was constrained. The 3rd experiment demonstrated that a concurrent presentation of identical auditory and visual technical text (without the presence of diagrams) was significantly less efficient in comparison with an auditory-only text. Actual or potential applications of this research include the design and evaluation of multimedia instructional systems and audiovisual displays.

[1]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  Managing split-attention and redundancy in multimedia instruction , 1999 .

[2]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory and Instructional Design: Recent Developments , 2003 .

[3]  R. Mayer,et al.  A Split-Attention Effect in Multimedia Learning: Evidence for Dual Processing Systems in Working Memory , 1998 .

[4]  C. Penney Modality effects and the structure of short-term verbal memory , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[5]  P. Chandler,et al.  Why Some Material Is Difficult to Learn , 1994 .

[6]  R. Mayer,et al.  Animations need narrations : an experimental test of a dual-coding hypothesis , 1991 .

[7]  N. Hari Narayanan,et al.  Multimedia instruction: lessons from evaluation of a theory-based design , 1999 .

[8]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design , 1998 .

[9]  Fred G. W. C. Paas,et al.  The Efficiency of Instructional Conditions: An Approach to Combine Mental Effort and Performance Measures , 1992 .

[10]  Paul Chandler,et al.  Levels of Expertise and Instructional Design , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[11]  R. Mayer,et al.  For whom is a picture worth a thousand words? Extensions of a dual-coding theory of multimedia learning. , 1994 .

[12]  R. Mayer,et al.  When learning is just a click away: Does simple user interaction foster deeper understanding of multimedia messages? , 2001 .

[13]  J. Sweller,et al.  Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes , 1995 .

[14]  Lawrence J. Najjar Multimedia information and learning , 1996 .

[15]  R. Mayer,et al.  Nine Ways to Reduce Cognitive Load in Multimedia Learning , 2003 .

[16]  R. Mayer,et al.  Cognitive Principles of Multimedia Learning: The Role of Modality and Contiguity , 1999 .

[17]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  The Expertise Reversal Effect , 2003 .

[18]  P. Chandler,et al.  Cognitive Load Theory and the Format of Instruction , 1991 .

[19]  Sigmar-Olaf Tergan,et al.  Misleading theoretical assumptions in hypertext/hypermedia research , 1997 .

[20]  Alfred Bork,et al.  Multimedia in Learning , 2001 .

[21]  R. Mayer,et al.  A generative theory of textbook design: Using annotated illustrations to foster meaningful learning of science text , 1995 .

[22]  A. Parkin,et al.  Human memory , 1999, Current Biology.

[23]  Jan van der Meij,et al.  Learning with Multiple Representations , 1998 .

[24]  D. Leutner,et al.  Direct Measurement of Cognitive Load in Multimedia Learning , 2003 .

[25]  Barbara Beccue,et al.  The Effects of Adding Audio Instructions to a Multimedia Computer Based Training Environment , 2001 .

[26]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Load Measurement as a Means to Advance Cognitive Load Theory , 2003 .

[27]  R. Mayer,et al.  The instructive animation: helping students build connections between words and pictures in multimedia learning , 1992 .

[28]  F. Paas Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. , 1992 .

[29]  J. Sweller Evolution of human cognitive architecture , 2003 .

[30]  R. Mayer,et al.  Multimedia Learning: The Promise of Multimedia Learning , 2001 .

[31]  R. Mayer,et al.  Verbal redundancy in multimedia learning: When reading helps listening , 2002 .

[32]  F. Paas,et al.  Variability of Worked Examples and Transfer of Geometrical Problem-Solving Skills: A Cognitive-Load Approach , 1994 .

[33]  R. Mayer,et al.  Cognitive constraints on multimedia learning: When presenting more material results in less understanding. , 2001 .

[34]  K. A. Ericsson,et al.  Long-term working memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[35]  Jeroen J. G. van Merriënboer,et al.  The modality effect in multimedia instructions , 2001 .

[36]  Slava Kalyuga,et al.  Incorporating Learner Experience into the Design of Multimedia Instruction. , 2000 .

[37]  Sharon K Tindall-Ford,et al.  When two sensory modes are better than one , 1997 .

[38]  John Sweller,et al.  Instructional Design in Technical Areas , 1999 .

[39]  Mark C. Detweiler,et al.  A Connectionist/Control Architecture for Working Memory , 1988 .

[40]  John Sweller,et al.  Cognitive load theory and instructional design , 1997 .