Effects of orientation on the identification of rotated objects depend on the level of identity.

Matching names and rotated line drawings of objects showed effects of object orientation that depended on name level. Large effects, in the same range as object naming, were found for rotations between 0 degrees and 120 degrees from upright with subordinate names (e.g., collie), whereas nonsignificant effects were found with superordinate (e.g., animal) and basic names (e.g., dog). These results support image normalization, after contact with orientation-invariant representations, that provide basic-level identity. They consequently fail to support theories of object recognition in which rotated object images are normalized to the upright position before contact with long-term object representations.

[1]  A. Taylor,et al.  The contribution of the right parietal lobe to object recognition. , 1973, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[2]  G. Keppel,et al.  Design and Analysis: A Researcher's Handbook , 1976 .

[3]  R. Shepard,et al.  CHRONOMETRIC STUDIES OF THE ROTATION OF MENTAL IMAGES , 1973 .

[4]  Irvin Rock,et al.  Orientation and form , 1974 .

[5]  W. Chase,et al.  Visual information processing. , 1974 .

[6]  S. Guttenplan Mind and language , 1975 .

[7]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  L. Cooper,et al.  Mental transformations and visual comparison processes: effects of complexity and similarity. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  Lynn A. Cooper,et al.  Individual differences in visual comparison processes , 1976 .

[10]  M. Corballis,et al.  Latency to categorize disoriented alphanumeric characters as letters or digits. , 1978 .

[11]  M. Corballis,et al.  Decisions about identity and orientation of rotated letters and digits , 1978, Memory & cognition.

[12]  D. Marr,et al.  Representation and recognition of the spatial organization of three-dimensional shapes , 1978, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[13]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[14]  G. Logan,et al.  The role of mental rotation in letter processing by children and adults. , 1980, Canadian journal of psychology.

[15]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory , 1980 .

[16]  M. G. Eley,et al.  Identifying rotated letter-like symbols , 1982, Memory & cognition.

[17]  R. Shepard,et al.  Mental Images and Their Transformations , 1982 .

[18]  S. Pinker,et al.  Visual cognition : An introduction * , 1989 .

[19]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Routes to Object Constancy: Implications from Neurological Impairments of Object Constancy , 1984, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[20]  Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al.  Pictures and names: Making the connection , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[21]  P. Jolicoeur The time to name disoriented natural objects , 1985, Memory & cognition.

[22]  M. Corballis,et al.  The naming of disoriented letters by normal and reading-disabled children. , 1985, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[23]  H. Brownell,et al.  Category differentiation in object recognition: typicality constraints on the basic category advantage. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  S. Carey,et al.  Why faces are and are not special: an effect of expertise. , 1986 .

[25]  E. Warrington,et al.  Visual Object Recognition in Patients with Right-Hemisphere Lesions: Axes or Features? , 1986, Perception.

[26]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[27]  Andrew Heathcote,et al.  Screen control and timing routines for the IBM microcomputer family using a high-level language , 1988 .

[28]  M. Corballis Recognition of disoriented shapes. , 1988, Psychological review.

[29]  John Crosbie,et al.  A simple Turbo Pascal 4.0 program for millisecond timing on the IBM PC/XT/AT , 1989 .

[30]  S. Ullman Aligning pictorial descriptions: An approach to object recognition , 1989, Cognition.

[31]  J. Norman,et al.  Why is word recognition impaired by disorientation while the identification of single letters is not? , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[32]  Y. Takano Perception of rotated forms: A theory of information types , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[33]  Marc Brysbaert,et al.  A warning about millisecond timing in Turbo Pascal , 1990 .

[34]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  The spatial frame of reference in object naming and discrimination of left-right reflections , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[35]  M. Tarr,et al.  When does Human Object Recognition use a Viewer-Centered Reference Frame? , 1990 .

[36]  Pierre Jolicoeur,et al.  Identification of Disoriented Objects: A Dual‐systems Theory , 1990 .

[37]  Michael J. Tarr,et al.  Article Commentary: Orientation-Dependent Mechanisms in Shape Recognition: Further Issues , 1991 .

[38]  M. Farah,et al.  Viewer-Centered and Object-Centered Representations in the Recognition of Naturalistic Line Drawings , 1991 .

[39]  I. Biederman,et al.  Dynamic binding in a neural network for shape recognition. , 1992, Psychological review.

[40]  Patricia A. McMullen,et al.  Reference frame and effects of orientation on finding the tops of rotated objects. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[41]  I. Biederman,et al.  Recognizing depth-rotated objects: Evidence and conditions for three-dimensional viewpoint invariance. , 1993 .

[42]  J. Hummel Reference Frames and Relations in Computational Models of Object Recognition , 1994 .

[43]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  A Solution to the Effect of Sample Size on Outlier Elimination , 1994 .

[44]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  Rotated object identification with and without orientation cues. , 1995, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[45]  J. Murray,et al.  Flipping and spinning: Spatial transformation procedures in the identification of rotated natural objects , 1997, Memory & cognition.