Effects of competitive prey capture on flight behavior and sonar beam pattern in paired big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus

SUMMARY Foraging and flight behavior of echolocating bats were quantitatively analyzed in this study. Paired big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, competed for a single food item in a large laboratory flight room. Their sonar beam patterns and flight paths were recorded by a microphone array and two high-speed cameras, respectively. Bats often remained in nearly classical pursuit (CP) states when one bat is following another bat. A follower can detect and anticipate the movement of the leader, while the leader has the advantage of gaining access to the prey first. Bats in the trailing position throughout the trial were more successful in accessing the prey. In this study, bats also used their sonar beam to monitor the conspecific's movement and to track the prey. Each bat tended to use its sonar beam to track the prey when it was closer to the worm than to another bat. The trailing bat often directed its sonar beam toward the leading bat in following flight. When two bats flew towards each other, they tended to direct their sonar beam axes away from each other, presumably to avoid signal jamming. This study provides a new perspective on how echolocating bats use their biosonar system to coordinate their flight with conspecifics in a group and how they compete for the same food source with conspecifics.

[1]  Jochen Zeil,et al.  The territorial flight of male houseflies (Fannia canicularis L.) , 1986, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[2]  J. Hutchinson Animal groups in three dimensions , 1999 .

[3]  M. Fenton,et al.  Exploiting vulnerable prey: moths and red bats (Lasiurus borealis; Vespertilionidae) , 2003 .

[4]  Puduru Viswanadha Reddy Steering laws for pursuit , 2007 .

[5]  C. Moss,et al.  The sonar beam pattern of a flying bat as it tracks tethered insects. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  Tomaso Poggio,et al.  Tracking and chasing in houseflies (Musca) , 1982, Biological Cybernetics.

[7]  Nachum Ulanovsky,et al.  Rapid jamming avoidance in biosonar , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[8]  Craig W. Reynolds Competition, Coevolution and the Game of Tag , 1994 .

[9]  E. W. Justh,et al.  Natural frames and interacting particles in three dimensions , 2005, Proceedings of the 44th IEEE Conference on Decision and Control.

[10]  Margrit Betke,et al.  Tracking Large Variable Numbers of Objects in Clutter , 2007, 2007 IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition.

[11]  M. Obrist Flexible bat echolocation: the influence of individual, habitat and conspecifics on sonar signal design , 1995, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[12]  Paul Glendinning,et al.  The mathematics of motion camouflage , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  Murray S. Klamkin,et al.  Cyclic Pursuit or "The Three Bugs Problems" , 1971 .

[14]  G. M. Hope,et al.  Electrical response of bat retina to spectral stimulation: comparison of four microchiropteran species , 1979, Experientia.

[15]  W. M. Masters,et al.  The degradation of distance discrimination in big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) caused by different interference signals , 1996, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[16]  James A Simmons,et al.  Jamming avoidance response of big brown bats in target detection , 2008, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[17]  James A. Simmons,et al.  Auditory Dimensions of Acoustic Images in Echolocation , 1995 .

[18]  P.V. Reddy,et al.  Motion camouflage in three dimensions , 2006, Proceedings of the 45th IEEE Conference on Decision and Control.

[19]  Cynthia F Moss,et al.  Echolocating Bats Use a Nearly Time-Optimal Strategy to Intercept Prey , 2006, PLoS biology.

[20]  M. Fenton,et al.  Foraging by red bats (Lasiurus borealis): do intraspecific chases mean territoriality? , 1990 .

[21]  H. L. Short,et al.  Mexican Free‐Tailed Bats in Texas , 1962 .

[22]  C. Wehrhahn,et al.  Sex-specific differences in the chasing behaviour of houseflies (Musca) , 1979, Biological Cybernetics.

[23]  C. Moss,et al.  Steering by Hearing: A Bat’s Acoustic Gaze Is Linked to Its Flight Motor Output by a Delayed, Adaptive Linear Law , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[24]  K. A. Raver,et al.  Range discrimination by big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) using altered model echoes: implications for signal processing. , 2000, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[25]  C. Moss,et al.  Flying in silence: Echolocating bats cease vocalizing to avoid sonar jamming , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[26]  James A. Simmons,et al.  Versatility of biosonar in the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus , 2001 .

[27]  C. Moss,et al.  Acoustic scanning of natural scenes by echolocation in the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus , 2009, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[28]  Wei Xian,et al.  Adaptive echolocation behavior in bats for the analysis of auditory scenes , 2009, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[29]  Ermin Wei,et al.  Pursuit and an evolutionary game , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences.

[30]  P. S. Krishnaprasad,et al.  Steering laws for motion camouflage , 2005, Proceedings of the Royal Society A: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences.

[31]  Iztok Lebar Bajec,et al.  Organized flight in birds , 2009, Animal Behaviour.

[32]  Peter Diehl,et al.  Radiotelemetric monitoring of heart-rate responses to song playback in blackbirds (Turdus merula) , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[33]  Bart Kranstauber,et al.  Group Hunting—A Reason for Sociality in Molossid Bats? , 2010, PloS one.

[34]  N. Ulanovsky,et al.  Dynamics of jamming avoidance in echolocating bats , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.