Syntactic rules in avian vocal sequences as a window into the evolution of compositionality
暂无分享,去创建一个
David Wheatcroft | Michael Griesser | Toshitaka N. Suzuki | D. Wheatcroft | M. Griesser | Toshitaka N. Suzuki
[1] Selvino R. de Kort,et al. Blackcaps (Sylvia atricapilla) increase the whistle part of their song in response to simulated territorial intrusion , 2015 .
[2] Thierry Aubin,et al. Does true syntax or simple auditory object support the role of skylark song dialect? , 2013, Animal Behaviour.
[3] Klaus Zuberbühler,et al. Conceptual Semantics in a Nonhuman Primate , 1999 .
[4] Andrea M.-K. Bierema,et al. On the Meaning of Alarm Calls: A Review of Functional Reference in Avian Alarm Calling , 2013 .
[5] W. Fitch. The Evolution of Language , 2010 .
[6] Timothy J. O'Donnell,et al. Evolutionary Linguistics: A New Look at an Old Landscape , 2007 .
[7] Dominique A. Potvin,et al. Birds orient their heads appropriately in response to functionally referential alarm calls of heterospecifics , 2018, Animal Behaviour.
[8] Sabrina Engesser,et al. Meaningful call combinations and compositional processing in the southern pied babbler , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[9] Douglas G. Smith,et al. An Investigation of Song-Based Species Recognition in the Red-Winged Blackbird (Agelaius Phoeniceus) , 1980 .
[10] Johan J Bolhuis,et al. Meaningful syntactic structure in songbird vocalizations? , 2018, PLoS biology.
[11] David Wheatcroft,et al. Call combinations in birds and the evolution of compositional syntax , 2018, PLoS biology.
[12] David Wheatcroft,et al. Wild Birds Use anOrdering Rule to DecodeNovel Call Sequences Graphical Abstract Highlights , 2017 .
[13] Marisa Hoeschele,et al. Neural Correlates of Threat Perception: Neural Equivalence of Conspecific and Heterospecific Mobbing Calls Is Learned , 2011, PloS one.
[14] Stephen Pruett-Jones,et al. Danger may enhance communication: predator calls alert females to male displays , 2010 .
[15] David Wheatcroft,et al. Species replacement reduces community participation in avian antipredator groups , 2016 .
[16] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Communication about predator type by a bird using discrete, graded and combinatorial variation in alarm calls , 2014, Animal Behaviour.
[17] J. C. Bremond,et al. Recherches sur la sémantique et les éléments vecteurs d’information dans les signaux acoustiques du rouge-gorge (Erithacus Rubecula L.) , 1968, La Terre et La Vie, Revue d'Histoire naturelle.
[18] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Referential mobbing calls elicit different predator-searching behaviours in Japanese great tits , 2012, Animal Behaviour.
[19] Robert D. Magrath,et al. Calling in the Face of Danger: Predation Risk and Acoustic Communication by Parent Birds and Their Offspring , 2010 .
[20] H. Gelter,et al. Song differences between the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca, the Collared Flycatcher F. albicollis, and their hybrids , 1987 .
[21] Koji Fujita,et al. Recursive Merge and Human Language Evolution , 2014 .
[22] Bernard Lohr,et al. Call learning in black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus) : The role of experience in the development of 'chick-a-dee' calls , 2010 .
[23] J. Hailman,et al. The ‘chick-a-dee’ calls of Parus atricapillus: A recombinant system of animal communication compared with written English , 1985 .
[24] Michael Griesser,et al. Referential Calls Signal Predator Behavior in a Group-Living Bird Species , 2008, Current Biology.
[25] R. Zann,et al. Structure, sequence and evolution of song elements in wild Australian Zebra finches , 1993 .
[26] Klaus Zuberbühler,et al. Language evolution: Semantic combinations in primate calls , 2006, Nature.
[27] M. Ryan,et al. What do animal signals mean? , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[28] L. Garamszegi,et al. Flycatcher song in allopatry and sympatry – convergence, divergence and reinforcement , 2004, Journal of evolutionary biology.
[29] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Assessment of predation risk through referential communication in incubating birds , 2015, Scientific Reports.
[30] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Calling at a Food Source: Context-Dependent Variation in Note Composition of Combinatorial Calls in Willow Tits , 2012 .
[31] April M. Becker,et al. MOBBING CALLS OF BLACK-CAPPED CHICKADEES: EFFECTS OF URGENCY ON CALL PRODUCTION , 2002 .
[32] R. Berwick,et al. Songs to syntax: the linguistics of birdsong , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[33] F. J. Pelletier. The Principle of Semantic Compositionality , 1994 .
[34] Torben Dabelsteen,et al. Song Features Essential for Species Discrimination and Behaviour Assessment By Male Blackbirds (Turdus Merula) , 1992 .
[35] Simon W. Townsend,et al. Functionally Referential Communication in Mammals: The Past, Present and the Future , 2013 .
[36] A. Kozhevnikov,et al. Temperature Manipulation in Songbird Brain Implicates the Premotor Nucleus HVC in Birdsong Syntax , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[37] P. Marler,et al. Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. , 1980, Science.
[38] Lise Menn,et al. Methods for studying language production , 1999 .
[39] Toshitaka N Suzuki,et al. From bird calls to human language: exploring the evolutionary drivers of compositional syntax , 2018, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.
[40] Richard Bertram,et al. Independent Premotor Encoding of the Sequence and Structure of Birdsong in Avian Cortex , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[41] Klaus Zuberbühler,et al. Food-associated calling sequences in bonobos , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[42] K. Okanoya. The Bengalese Finch: A Window on the Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong Syntax , 2004, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[43] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Long-Distance Calling by the Willow Tit, Poecile montanus, Facilitates Formation of Mixed-Species Foraging Flocks , 2012 .
[44] David Wheatcroft,et al. Reproductive interference via display signals: the challenge of multiple receivers , 2015, Population Ecology.
[45] Timothy Q. Gentner,et al. Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds , 2006, Nature.
[46] M. Manser,et al. The acoustic structure of suricates' alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[47] K. Zuberbühler,et al. Campbell's monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[48] Noam Chomsky,et al. वाक्यविन्यास का सैद्धान्तिक पक्ष = Aspects of the theory of syntax , 1965 .
[49] A. Qvarnström,et al. Song similarity predicts hybridization in flycatchers , 2006, Journal of evolutionary biology.
[50] Raoul A. Mulder,et al. A Novel Context for Bird Song: Predator Calls Prompt Male Singing in the Kleptogamous Superb Fairy‐wren, Malurus cyaneus , 2010 .
[51] Noam Chomsky,et al. Minimalist inquiries : the framework , 1998 .
[52] R. Seyfarth,et al. The assessment by vervet monkeys of their own and another species' alarm calls , 1990, Animal Behaviour.
[53] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Parental alarm calls warn nestlings about different predatory threats , 2011, Current Biology.
[54] L. Wallin,et al. Integration of a call into the song of the Collared Flycatcher: adaptive compensation for booadcast efficiency? , 1987 .
[55] David Wheatcroft,et al. Repetition rate of calls used in multiple contexts communicates presence of predators to nestlings and adult birds , 2015, Animal Behaviour.
[56] K. Zuberbühler. Referential Signaling in Non-Human Primates: Cognitive Precursors and Limitations for the Evolution of Language , 2003 .
[57] Erick Greene,et al. Allometry of Alarm Calls: Black-Capped Chickadees Encode Information About Predator Size , 2005, Science.
[58] Toshitaka N Suzuki,et al. Experimental evidence for compositional syntax in bird calls , 2016, Nature Communications.
[59] M. C. Baker,et al. VOCAL ONTOGENY OF NESTLING AND FLEDGLING BLACK-CAPPED CHICKADEES POECILE ATRICAPILLA IN NATURAL POPULATIONS , 2003 .
[60] Klaus Zuberbühler,et al. Causal knowledge of predators' behaviour in wild Diana monkeys , 2000, Animal Behaviour.
[61] K. Riebel,et al. Are good ornaments bad armaments? Male chaffinch perception of songs with varying flourish length , 2003, Animal Behaviour.
[62] Todd M. Freeberg,et al. Chick-a-dee call variation in Carolina chickadees and recruiting flockmates to food , 2009 .
[63] Noam Chomsky,et al. The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? , 2002 .
[64] K A Shiovitz,et al. The process of species-specific song recognition by the indigo bunting, Passerina cyanea, and its relationship to the organization of avian acoustical behavior. , 1975, Behaviour.
[65] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Semantic communication in birds: evidence from field research over the past two decades , 2016, Ecological Research.
[66] E. Balaban,et al. Bird song syntax: learned intraspecific variation is meaningful. , 1988, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[67] Gábor Győri. Animal communication and human language , 1995 .
[68] Nobuyuki Kutsukake,et al. Foraging intention affects whether willow tits call to attract members of mixed-species flocks , 2017, Royal Society Open Science.
[69] Willem H. Zuidema,et al. Simple rules can explain discrimination of putative recursive syntactic structures by a songbird species , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[70] N. Davies,et al. Learning fine-tunes a specific response of nestlings to the parental alarm calls of their own species , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[71] K. Zuberbühler,et al. Meaningful call combinations in a non-human primate , 2008, Current Biology.
[72] P. Slater,et al. Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations , 1995 .
[73] Jeffrey Podos,et al. The evolution of bird song: male and female response to song innovation in swamp sparrows , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[74] Todd M Freeberg,et al. Receivers respond differently to chick-a-dee calls varying in note composition in Carolina chickadees, Poecile carolinensis , 2002, Animal Behaviour.
[75] B. Partee,et al. Mathematical Methods in Linguistics , 1990 .
[76] Toshitaka N. Suzuki. Alarm calls evoke a visual search image of a predator in birds , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[77] Jeffrey R. Lucas,et al. Chick-a-dee call syntax, social context, and season affect vocal responses of Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis) , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.
[78] Svein Haftorn. Ontogeny of the vocal repertoire in the Willow Tit Parus montanus , 1993 .
[79] Marc F. Schmidt,et al. At the interface of the auditory and vocal motor systems: NIf and its role in vocal processing, production and learning , 2013, Journal of Physiology-Paris.
[80] M. Manser,et al. The information that receivers extract from alarm calls in suricates , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[81] Todd M. Freeberg,et al. Complexity in the Chick-a-Dee Call of Carolina Chickadees (Poecile Carolinensis): Associations of Context and Signaler Behavior to Call Structure , 2008 .
[82] Stephen T. Emlen,et al. An Experimental Analysis of the Parameters of Bird Song Eliciting Species Recognition , 1972 .
[83] Andrew N. Radford,et al. The development of alarm call behaviour in mammals and birds , 2009, Animal Behaviour.
[84] Robert W. Ficken,et al. VOCAL REPERTOIRE OF THE BLACK-CAPPED CHICKADEE , 1978 .
[85] Carel ten Cate,et al. Zebra Finch Song Phonology and Syntactical Structure across Populations and Continents—A Computational Comparison , 2016, Front. Psychol..
[86] Jack P. Hailman,et al. Combinatorial animal communication with computable syntax: Chick-a-dee calling qualifies as ‘Language’ by structural linguistics , 1986, Animal Behaviour.
[87] Torben Dabelsteen,et al. Coding in the song of the wren: importance of rhythmicity, syntax and element structure , 2000, Animal Behaviour.