From partners to populations: A hierarchical Bayesian account of coordination and convention

away to a smaller number of more general terms; in the fine and mixed conditions, however, agents become more confident of subordinate terms. Partners successfully learn to communicate First, we compare the model’s learning curves across context conditions (Fig. 10A). We focus on the coarse and fine conditions for simplicity, since this single comparison captures the core phenomena of interest. In a mixed-effects logic regression, we find that communicative accuracy steadily improves over time across all conditions, b = 0.72, z = 16.9, p < 0.001. However, accuracy also differed across conditions: adding a main effect of condition significantly improves model fit, χ2(2) = 9.6, p = 0.008. Accuracy is significantly higher in the coarse condition than the fine condition b = −0.71, z = 9.3, p < 0.001 and marginally higher than the mixed condition. Lexical conventions are shaped by context As an initial marker of context sensitivity, we examine the effective vocabulary sizes used by speakers in each condition. We operationalized this measure by counting the total number of unique words produced within each repetition block. This measure takes a value of 8 when a different word is consistently used for every object, and a value of 1 when exactly the same word is used for every object. In an mixed-effects regression model including intercepts and random effects of trial number for each simulated trajectory, we find an overall main effect of condition, with agents in the fine condition using significantly fewer words across all repetition blocks (m = 4.7 in coarse, m = 6.5 in fine, t = 4.5, p < 0.001). However, we also found a significant interaction: the effective vocabulary size gradually increased over time in the fine condition, while it stayed roughly constant in in the coarse condition, b = 0.18, t = 8.1, p < 0.001, see Fig. 10B.

[1]  Luc Steels,et al.  A Self-Organizing Spatial Vocabulary , 1995, Artificial Life.

[2]  Herbert H. Clark,et al.  Coordinating beliefs in conversation , 1992 .

[3]  Raymond W. Gibbs,et al.  Beyond the Lexicon: Creativity in Language Production , 1988 .

[4]  Kristjan Kalm,et al.  Visual recency bias is explained by a mixture model of internal representations. , 2018, Journal of vision.

[5]  Moshe Tennenholtz,et al.  On the Emergence of Social Conventions: Modeling, Analysis, and Simulations , 1997, Artif. Intell..

[6]  A. Woodward,et al.  Sensitivity of 24-month-olds to the prior inaccuracy of the source: possible mechanisms. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[7]  C. Bicchieri The grammar of society: the nature and dynamics of social norms , 2005 .

[8]  Roberto A. Weber,et al.  Cultural Conflict and Merger Failure: An Experimental Approach , 2003, Manag. Sci..

[9]  P. Bloom How children learn the meanings of words , 2000 .

[10]  V. Ferreira,et al.  Syntactic Entrainment: The Repetition of Syntactic Structures in Event Descriptions. , 2019, Journal of memory and language.

[11]  Noga Zaslavsky,et al.  A Rate–Distortion view of human pragmatic reasoning? , 2020, SCIL.

[12]  S. Brown-Schmidt,et al.  Partner-specific interpretation of maintained referential precedents during interactive dialog. , 2009, Journal of memory and language.

[13]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  Disentangling contributions of visual information and interaction history in the formation of graphical conventions , 2019, CogSci.

[14]  P. Eckert Three Waves of Variation Study: The Emergence of Meaning in the Study of Sociolinguistic Variation , 2012 .

[15]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  Recasting Gradient-Based Meta-Learning as Hierarchical Bayes , 2018, ICLR.

[16]  E. Markman,et al.  Word learning in children: an examination of fast mapping. , 1987, Child development.

[17]  Simon Garrod,et al.  Joint Action, Interactive Alignment, and Dialog , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[18]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  Continual Adaptation for Efficient Machine Communication , 2019, CONLL.

[19]  Gareth Roberts,et al.  The emergence of duality of patterning: Insights from the laboratory , 2012, Language and Cognition.

[20]  Shiri Lev-Ari,et al.  Larger communities create more systematic languages , 2019, Proceedings of the Royal Society B.

[21]  George Lakoff,et al.  Hedges: A study in meaning criteria and the logic of fuzzy concepts , 1973, J. Philos. Log..

[22]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Bayesian Special Section Learning Overhypotheses with Hierarchical Bayesian Models , 2022 .

[23]  Ted Briscoe,et al.  Weakly Supervised Learning for Hedge Classification in Scientific Literature , 2007, ACL.

[24]  Eighteen-month-olds selectively generalize words from accurate speakers to novel contexts. , 2018, Developmental science.

[25]  Andrew Gordon Wilson,et al.  Subspace Inference for Bayesian Deep Learning , 2019, UAI.

[26]  Nicolas Gauvrit,et al.  Adults are more efficient in creating and transmitting novel signalling systems than children , 2019, Journal of Language Evolution.

[27]  Shiri Lev-Ari,et al.  Talking to fewer people leads to having more malleable linguistic representations , 2017, PloS one.

[28]  Christopher Potts,et al.  Pragmatically Informative Image Captioning with Character-Level Inference , 2018, NAACL.

[29]  T. Brochhagen Brief at the Risk of Being Misunderstood: Consolidating Population- and Individual-Level Tendencies , 2021 .

[30]  Jon Oberlander,et al.  Foundations of Representation: Where Might Graphical Symbol Systems Come From? , 2007, Cogn. Sci..

[31]  Mike Frank,et al.  Convention-formation in iterated reference games , 2017, CogSci.

[32]  V. Ferreira,et al.  Do priming effects in dialogue reflect partner- or task-based expectations? , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[33]  The Pervasive Role of Pragmatics in Early Language , 2019 .

[34]  Martin J. Pickering,et al.  Alignment as the Basis for Successful Communication , 2006 .

[35]  Amy Perfors,et al.  Language learning, language use and the evolution of linguistic variation , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[36]  Johan Kwisthout,et al.  Cognition and intractability: a guide to classical and parameterized complexity analysis , 2019 .

[37]  Robert D. Hawkins,et al.  Learning to communicate about shared procedural abstractions , 2021, ArXiv.

[38]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Creating a communication system from scratch: gesture beats vocalization hands down , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[39]  L. Steels,et al.  In-depth analysis of the Naming Game dynamics: the homogeneous mixing case , 2008, ArXiv.

[40]  Terry Regier,et al.  The Emergence of Words: Attentional Learning in Form and Meaning , 2005, Cogn. Sci..

[41]  Michael Tomasello,et al.  How toddlers and preschoolers learn to uniquely identify referents for others: a training study. , 2007, Child development.

[42]  Simon Kirby,et al.  Minimal Requirements for the Emergence of Learned Signaling , 2014, Cogn. Sci..

[43]  A. Roth,et al.  Predicting How People Play Games: Reinforcement Learning in Experimental Games with Unique, Mixed Strategy Equilibria , 1998 .

[44]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Ad hoc categories , 1983, Memory & cognition.

[45]  Iris van Rooij,et al.  The Tractable Cognition Thesis , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[46]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Individual Choice Behavior: A Theoretical Analysis , 1979 .

[47]  Ivar Kolstad The evolution of social norms , 2003 .

[48]  Daniel L. K. Yamins,et al.  Common Object Representations for Visual Production and Recognition , 2018, Cogn. Sci..

[49]  R. Krauss,et al.  Changes in reference phrases as a function of frequency of usage in social interaction: a preliminary study , 1964 .

[50]  Mark Dingemanse,et al.  A simple repair mechanism can alleviate computational demands of pragmatic reasoning: simulations and complexity analysis , 2020, CoNLL.

[51]  William M. Smith,et al.  A Study of Thinking , 1956 .

[52]  Judith Degen,et al.  Modeling cross-linguistic production of referring expressions , 2021, SCIL.

[53]  Mari Ostendorf,et al.  Low-Rank RNN Adaptation for Context-Aware Language Modeling , 2017, TACL.

[54]  Dan Klein,et al.  Reasoning about Pragmatics with Neural Listeners and Speakers , 2016, EMNLP.

[55]  Dave F. Kleinschmidt,et al.  Robust speech perception: recognize the familiar, generalize to the similar, and adapt to the novel. , 2015, Psychological review.

[56]  D. Schiffrin Meaning, form, and use in context : linguistic applications , 1984 .

[57]  Richard Warner,et al.  Philosophical Grounds of Rationality: Intentions, Categories, Ends , 1986 .

[58]  Robert Dale,et al.  Computational Interpretations of the Gricean Maxims in the Generation of Referring Expressions , 1995, Cogn. Sci..

[59]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  The role of gesture and mimetic representation in making language the province of speech. , 1999 .

[60]  付伶俐 打磨Using Language,倡导新理念 , 2014 .

[61]  Eve V. Clark,et al.  The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. , 1987 .

[62]  V. Ferreira,et al.  Repeat After Us: Syntactic Alignment is Not Partner-Specific. , 2019, Journal of memory and language.

[63]  Jasper Snoek,et al.  Efficient and Scalable Bayesian Neural Nets with Rank-1 Factors , 2020, ICML.

[64]  Christopher Potts,et al.  Emergence of Gricean Maxims from Multi-Agent Decision Theory , 2013, NAACL.

[65]  Gareth Roberts,et al.  An experimental study of social selection and frequency of interaction in linguistic diversity , 2010 .

[66]  Armin W. Schulz Signals: evolution, learning, and information , 2012 .

[67]  Gerhard Jäger,et al.  The evolution of convex categories , 2007 .

[68]  Josh P. Armstrong The problem of lexical innovation , 2016, Linguistics and Philosophy.

[69]  H. Young The Economics of Convention , 1996 .

[70]  Katherine D. Kinzler Language as a Social Cue. , 2021, Annual review of psychology.

[71]  S. Asher,et al.  Children's Failure to Communicate: An Assessment of Comparison and Egocentrism Explanations. , 1976 .

[72]  Roger Levy,et al.  Pragmatic reasoning through semantic inference , 2016, Semantics and Pragmatics.

[73]  Rick Dale,et al.  Behavior Matching in Multimodal Communication Is Synchronized , 2012, Cogn. Sci..

[74]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Understanding by addressees and overhearers , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[75]  Siobhan Chapman Logic and Conversation , 2005 .

[76]  E. Schegloff,et al.  The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation , 1977 .

[77]  L. Steels Modeling the cultural evolution of language. , 2011, Physics of life reviews.

[78]  N. Chater,et al.  Unwritten rules: virtual bargaining underpins social interaction, culture, and society , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[79]  Judith Degen,et al.  When redundancy is useful: A Bayesian approach to "overinformative" referring expressions. , 2020, Psychological review.

[80]  Lael J. Schooler,et al.  The Adaptive Nature of Memory , 2017 .

[81]  Bruno Galantucci,et al.  An Experimental Study of the Emergence of Human Communication Systems , 2005, Cogn. Sci..

[82]  Kees van Deemter Computational Models of Referring: A Study in Cognitive Science , 2016 .

[83]  Bruce Fraser,et al.  Pragmatic Competence: The Case of Hedging , 2010 .

[84]  Rachel Ryskin,et al.  People as contexts in conversation , 2015 .

[85]  Richard J. Gerrig,et al.  Revisiting the Memory-Based Processing Approach to Common Ground , 2016, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[86]  William S Horton,et al.  The influence of partner-specific memory associations on language production: Evidence from picture naming , 2007, Language and cognitive processes.

[87]  P. König,et al.  Reinforcement of Semantic Representations in Pragmatic Agents Leads to the Emergence of a Mutual Exclusivity Bias , 2020, CogSci.

[88]  M. Tanenhaus,et al.  The Flexibility of Conceptual Pacts: Referring Expressions Dynamically Shift to Accommodate New Conceptualizations , 2016, Front. Psychol..

[89]  Mark Steyvers,et al.  Online Learning Mechanisms for Bayesian Models of Word Segmentation , 2010 .

[90]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Using Speakers ’ Referential Intentions to Model Early Cross-Situational Word Learning , 2022 .

[91]  James R. Hurford,et al.  Biological evolution of the Saussurean sign as a component of the language acquisition device , 1989 .

[92]  Roger Levy,et al.  That's what she (could have) said: How alternative utterances affect language use , 2012, CogSci.

[93]  Michael Franke,et al.  Variations on a Bayesian Theme: Comparing Bayesian Models of Referential Reasoning , 2015 .

[94]  J. Siskind A computational study of cross-situational techniques for learning word-to-meaning mappings , 1996, Cognition.

[95]  THE REALITY OF LANGUAGE , 2010 .

[96]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Bayesian Learning for Neural Networks , 1995 .

[97]  H. Giles,et al.  Contexts of Accommodation: Developments in Applied Sociolinguistics , 2010 .

[98]  Laura Graesser,et al.  Emergent Linguistic Phenomena in Multi-Agent Communication Games , 2019, EMNLP.

[99]  A. Backus Code-switching in conversation: Language, interaction and identity , 2000 .

[100]  Computational challenges in explaining communication: How deep the rabbit hole goes , 2021 .

[101]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Characterizing the dynamics of learning in repeated reference games , 2020, Cogn. Sci..

[102]  Ines Gloeckner,et al.  Relevance Communication And Cognition , 2016 .

[103]  S. Gershman On the Blessing of Abstraction , 2017, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[104]  Katherine D. Kinzler,et al.  The native language of social cognition , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[105]  Peter D. Bricker,et al.  The role of audible and visible back-channel responses in interpersonal communication. , 1977 .

[106]  S. Kirby,et al.  Compression and communication in the cultural evolution of linguistic structure , 2015, Cognition.

[107]  Tal Linzen,et al.  A Neural Model of Adaptation in Reading , 2018, EMNLP.

[108]  S. Rosenberg,et al.  Referential processes of speakers and listeners. , 1966, Psychological review.

[109]  Sean Roberts,et al.  Emergence of systematic iconicity: transmission, interaction and analogy , 2015, CogSci.

[110]  Knowing How You Know: Toddlers Reevaluate Words Learned From an Unreliable Speaker , 2021, Open Mind.

[111]  S. Kirby,et al.  Simplicity and informativeness in semantic category systems , 2020, Cognition.

[112]  S. Levinson Presumptive Meanings: The theory of generalized conversational implicature , 2001 .

[113]  M. Pickering,et al.  Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[114]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  The Language of Generalization , 2016, Psychological review.

[115]  Barbara G Deutsch,et al.  The Structure of Task Oriented Dialogs , 1974 .

[116]  Johan Kwisthout,et al.  Bayesian Intractability Is Not an Ailment That Approximation Can Cure , 2011, Cogn. Sci..

[117]  R. Kirk CONVENTION: A PHILOSOPHICAL STUDY , 1970 .

[118]  Christopher T. Kello,et al.  Complexity matching in dyadic conversation. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[119]  S. Garrod,et al.  Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination , 1987, Cognition.

[120]  J. Wixted,et al.  On the Form of Forgetting , 1991 .

[121]  Hyowon Gweon,et al.  Learning the Structure of Social Influence. , 2017, Cognitive science.

[122]  M. Tomasello,et al.  Young children spontaneously recreate core properties of language in a new modality , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[123]  Robert X D Hawkins,et al.  Conducting real-time multiplayer experiments on the web , 2014, Behavior Research Methods.

[124]  L. Steels,et al.  coordinating perceptually grounded categories through language: a case study for colour , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[125]  Joseph Hilbe,et al.  Data Analysis Using Regression and Multilevel/Hierarchical Models , 2009 .

[126]  Michael Franke,et al.  Bidirectional Optimization from Reasoning and Learning in Games , 2012, J. Log. Lang. Inf..

[127]  S. Brown-Schmidt,et al.  The Influence of Partner-Specific Memory Associations on Picture Naming: A Failure to Replicate Horton (2007) , 2014, PloS one.

[128]  Charles Kemp,et al.  Kinship Categories Across Languages Reflect General Communicative Principles , 2012, Science.

[129]  Adele E. Goldberg,et al.  Explain Me This , 2019 .

[130]  Hyowon Gweon,et al.  The rare preference effect: Statistical information influences social affiliation judgments , 2019, Cognition.

[131]  S. Brennan,et al.  When conceptual pacts are broken: Partner-specific effects on the comprehension of referring expressions , 2003 .

[132]  Kees van Deemter Computational Models of Referring , 2019, The Oxford Handbook of Reference.

[133]  Philip J. Cowans Information retrieval using hierarchical dirichlet processes , 2004, SIGIR '04.

[134]  Vittorio Loreto,et al.  Modeling the emergence of universality in color naming patterns , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[135]  Susan R. Fussell,et al.  The effects of intended audience on message production and comprehension: Reference in a common ground framework , 1989 .

[136]  D. Davidson,et al.  The Social Aspect of Language , 1994 .

[137]  Samuel Gershman,et al.  Novelty and Inductive Generalization in Human Reinforcement Learning , 2015, Top. Cogn. Sci..

[138]  Sang Joon Kim,et al.  A Mathematical Theory of Communication , 2006 .

[139]  Pieter Abbeel,et al.  Emergence of Grounded Compositional Language in Multi-Agent Populations , 2017, AAAI.

[140]  D. Davidson Communication and convention , 1984, Synthese.

[141]  Ichiro Umata,et al.  Graphical Language Games: Interactional Constraints on Representational Form , 2007, Cogn. Sci..

[142]  Thomas Brochhagen,et al.  Signalling under Uncertainty: Interpretative Alignment without a Common Prior , 2017, The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science.

[143]  S. Kirby,et al.  Evolving artificial sign languages in the lab: From improvised gesture to systematic sign , 2019, Cognition.

[144]  L. Steels,et al.  Agent-Based Models of Strategies for the Emergence and Evolution of Grammatical Agreement , 2013, PloS one.

[145]  R. Krauss,et al.  Effects of Transmission Delay and Access Delay on the Efficiency of Verbal Communication , 1967 .

[146]  E. Gibson,et al.  How Efficiency Shapes Human Language , 2019, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[147]  Bevil R. Conway,et al.  Color naming across languages reflects color use , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[148]  S. Gershman,et al.  Social-Structure Learning , 2020 .

[149]  Christopher Potts A case for deep learning in semantics: Response to Pater , 2019, Language.

[150]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Learning to Learn Causal Models , 2010, Cogn. Sci..

[151]  Michael Franke,et al.  Probabilistic pragmatics, or why Bayes’ rule is probably important for pragmatics , 2016 .

[152]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Sequential effects: Superstition or rational behavior? , 2008, NIPS.

[153]  D. Davidson,et al.  A Nice Derangement of Epitaphs , 2018 .

[154]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  Learning a theory of causality. , 2011, Psychological review.

[155]  Bruno Galantucci,et al.  How communication changes when we cannot mime the world: Experimental evidence for the effect of iconicity on combinatoriality , 2015, Cognition.

[156]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Estimating the sources of motor errors for adaptation and generalization , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[157]  S. Garrod,et al.  Conversation, co-ordination and convention: an empirical investigation of how groups establish linguistic conventions , 1994, Cognition.

[158]  Tyler Marghetis,et al.  Cognitive biases and social coordination in the emergence of temporal language , 2016, CogSci.

[159]  David J. C. MacKay,et al.  A Practical Bayesian Framework for Backpropagation Networks , 1992, Neural Computation.

[160]  Sandro Pezzelle,et al.  Refer, Reuse, Reduce: Generating Subsequent References in Visual and Conversational Contexts , 2020, EMNLP.

[161]  Steven T. Piantadosi,et al.  The communicative function of ambiguity in language , 2011, Cognition.

[162]  R. Nosofsky American Psychological Association, Inc. Choice, Similarity, and the Context Theory of Classification , 2022 .

[163]  Steven G. Luke,et al.  Evaluating significance in linear mixed-effects models in R , 2016, Behavior Research Methods.

[164]  Mike Wu,et al.  Pragmatic Inference and Visual Abstraction Enable Contextual Flexibility During Visual Communication , 2019, Computational Brain & Behavior.

[165]  Alexandre Lacoste,et al.  Uncertainty in Multitask Transfer Learning , 2018, ArXiv.

[166]  R. Gerrig,et al.  The impact of memory demands on audience design during language production , 2005, Cognition.

[167]  Susan Carey,et al.  Acquiring a Single New Word , 1978 .

[168]  H. H. Clark,et al.  Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[169]  Giosuè Baggio,et al.  Role Asymmetry and Code Transmission in Signaling Games: An Experimental and Computational Investigation , 2015, Cogn. Sci..

[170]  Y. Bi,et al.  Idiosyncratic Tower of Babel: Individual Differences in Word-Meaning Representation Increase as Word Abstractness Increases , 2021, Psychological science.

[171]  Todd Wareham,et al.  Why is scaling up models of language evolution hard? , 2021 .

[172]  R. Krauss,et al.  Referential communication in nursery school children: method and some preliminary findings. , 1966, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[173]  J Mullin,et al.  Interactive communication between children: learning how to make language work in dialogue , 1994, Journal of Child Language.

[174]  Helen Goodluck,et al.  First language acquisition. , 2011, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[175]  M. Studdert-Kennedy Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal About Thought. , 1994 .

[176]  Michel Hupet,et al.  Changes in repeated references: Collaboration or repetition effects? , 1992, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research.

[177]  Andrea Baronchelli,et al.  The spontaneous emergence of conventions: An experimental study of cultural evolution , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[178]  Michael Franke,et al.  Emerging abstractions: Lexical conventions are shaped by communicative context , 2018, CogSci.

[179]  M. Engelmann The Philosophical Investigations , 2013 .

[180]  H. H. Clark,et al.  References in Conversation Between Experts and Novices , 1987 .

[181]  M. Ross Quillian,et al.  Retrieval time from semantic memory , 1969 .

[182]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Learning and using language via recursive pragmatic reasoning about other agents , 2013, NIPS.

[183]  Dale J. Barr,et al.  Establishing conventional communication systems: Is common knowledge necessary? , 2004, Cogn. Sci..

[184]  T. Chartrand,et al.  Using Nonconscious Behavioral Mimicry to Create Affiliation and Rapport , 2003, Psychological science.

[185]  Thomas L. Griffiths,et al.  Generalizing meanings from partners to populations: Hierarchical inference supports convention formation on networks , 2020, CogSci.

[186]  Robert D. Hawkins,et al.  Parents scaffold the formation of conversational pacts with their children , 2020, CogSci.

[187]  Christopher Potts,et al.  An Incremental Iterated Response Model of Pragmatics , 2018, ArXiv.

[188]  Leonidas J. Guibas,et al.  Shapeglot: Learning Language for Shape Differentiation , 2019, 2019 IEEE/CVF International Conference on Computer Vision (ICCV).

[189]  Simon Garrod,et al.  The Interactive Evolution of Human Communication Systems , 2010, Cogn. Sci..

[190]  Ivan Toni,et al.  Conceptual Alignment: How Brains Achieve Mutual Understanding , 2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[191]  Xavier Seron,et al.  The effects of the codability and discriminability of the referents on the collaborative referring procedure , 1991 .

[192]  Petra Augurzky,et al.  Gricean Expectations in Online Sentence Comprehension: An ERP Study on the Processing of Scalar Inferences , 2019, Cogn. Sci..

[193]  Philip R. Cohen,et al.  Referring as a Collaborative Process , 2003 .

[194]  Adam N Sanborn,et al.  Exemplar models as a mechanism for performing Bayesian inference , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[195]  Kobin H. Kendrick,et al.  Universal Principles in the Repair of Communication Problems , 2015, PloS one.

[196]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  Nonliteral understanding of number words , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[197]  John M. Carroll,et al.  Naming and Describing in Social Communication , 1980 .

[198]  Candace L. Sidner,et al.  Attention, Intentions, and the Structure of Discourse , 1986, CL.

[199]  H. H. Clark Depicting as a method of communication. , 2016, Psychological review.

[200]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  Adjectival vagueness in a Bayesian model of interpretation , 2015, Synthese.

[201]  B. Mcguinness,et al.  Reply to Davidson , 1994 .

[202]  Roger Levy,et al.  Negotiating Lexical Uncertainty and Speaker Expertise with Disjunction , 2015 .

[203]  S. Kirby,et al.  Contextual predictability shapes signal autonomy , 2018, Cognition.

[204]  Sean M. Polyn,et al.  A context maintenance and retrieval model of organizational processes in free recall. , 2009, Psychological review.

[205]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Review Pragmatic Language Interpretation as Probabilistic Inference , 2022 .

[206]  M. Pickering,et al.  The Representation of Verbs: Evidence from Syntactic Priming in Language Production , 1998 .

[207]  D. Fudenberg,et al.  Recency, consistent learning, and Nash equilibrium , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[208]  G. Lupyan,et al.  Finding categories through words: More nameable features improve category learning , 2019, Cognition.

[209]  Terry Regier,et al.  Word Meanings across Languages Support Efficient Communication , 2015 .

[210]  Sam Glucksberg,et al.  The Development of Communication: Competence as a Function of Age. , 1969 .

[211]  Cynthia G. Clopper,et al.  Effects of Talker Variability on Perceptual Learning of Dialects , 2004, Language and speech.

[212]  Katherine D. Kinzler,et al.  Accent trumps race in guiding children's social preferences. , 2009, Social cognition.

[213]  Luc Steels,et al.  Agent-based models for the emergence and evolution of grammar , 2016, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[214]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Embedded Implicatures as Pragmatic Inferences under Compositional Lexical Uncertainty , 2015, J. Semant..

[215]  Simon Kirby,et al.  Systematicity and arbitrariness in novel communication systems , 2010 .

[216]  Dan Klein,et al.  Pragmatically Informative Text Generation , 2019, NAACL.

[217]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  The Formation of Social Conventions in Real-Time Environments , 2016, PloS one.

[218]  S. Glucksberg,et al.  WHAT DO PEOPLE SAY AFTER THEY HAVE LEARNED HOW TO TALK? STUDIES OF THE DEVELOPMENT OF REFERENTIAL COMMUNICATION1 , 2016 .

[219]  Chen Yu,et al.  The unrealized promise of infant statistical word–referent learning , 2014, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[220]  R. Krauss,et al.  Concurrent feedback, confirmation, and the encoding of referents in verbal communication. , 1966, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[221]  Stephanie Kelter,et al.  Surface form and memory in question answering , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[222]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  An integrative account of constraints on cross-situational learning , 2015, Cognition.

[223]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Word learning as Bayesian inference. , 2007, Psychological review.

[224]  Christopher Potts,et al.  Colors in Context: A Pragmatic Neural Model for Grounded Language Understanding , 2017, TACL.

[225]  Charles Kemp,et al.  How to Grow a Mind: Statistics, Structure, and Abstraction , 2011, Science.

[226]  Simon Kirby,et al.  Languages adapt to their contextual niche , 2014, Language and Cognition.

[227]  Learning from other minds: an optimistic critique of reinforcement learning models of social learning , 2021, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[228]  A. Roelofs,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking , 1992, Cognition.

[229]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  The Emergence of Social Norms and Conventions , 2019, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[230]  John K Kruschke,et al.  Bayesian data analysis. , 2010, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[231]  Osteitis Deformans ON A FORM , 2002 .

[232]  Gerhard Jäger,et al.  Language structure: psychological and social constraints , 2007, Synthese.

[233]  Sarah C. Creel,et al.  How Talker Identity Relates to Language Processing , 2011, Lang. Linguistics Compass.

[234]  Christopher Potts,et al.  A case for deep learning in semantics , 2018, ArXiv.

[235]  Yang Xu,et al.  Semantic Typology and Efficient Communication , 2018 .

[236]  Gemma Boleda,et al.  Which one is the dax? Achieving mutual exclusivity with neural networks , 2020, ArXiv.

[237]  Josh P. Armstrong Coordination, Triangulation, and Language Use , 2016 .

[238]  C. R.,et al.  On referring , 1950 .

[239]  Doina Precup,et al.  Shaping representations through communication: community size effect in artificial learning systems , 2019, ArXiv.

[240]  R. Krauss,et al.  Social and Nonsocial Speech , 1977 .

[241]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  Individual Differences in Language Acquisition and Processing , 2018, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[242]  Yoav Artzi,et al.  Analysis of Language Change in Collaborative Instruction Following , 2021, SCIL.

[243]  Ryan P. Adams,et al.  Probabilistic Backpropagation for Scalable Learning of Bayesian Neural Networks , 2015, ICML.

[244]  B. V. Fraassen Singular Terms, Truth-Value Gaps, and Free Logic , 1966 .

[245]  Susan Brennan,et al.  Partner-Specific Adaptation in Dialog , 2009, Top. Cogn. Sci..