Students' Representational Fluency at University: A Cross-Sectional Measure of How Multiple Representations Are Used by Physics Students Using the Representational Fluency Survey.

To succeed within scientific disciplines, using representations, including those based on words, graphs, equations, and diagrams, is important. Research indicates that the use of discipline specific representations (sometimes referred to as expert generated representations), as well as multi-representational use, is critical for problem solving and developing understanding. This paper consolidates these ideas using the Representational Fluency Survey (RFS) over two years with 334 students at The University of Sydney. Analysis shows that there was a significant difference between the representational fluency of the 1st year Fundamental and Regular students (low level 1st year physics courses) compared to the 1st year Advanced, 2nd year, 3rd year and Postgraduate level students. The existence of this distinct gap is further supported by evidence from qualitative coding that students with a high level of representational fluency use a greater number of representations and more visual and symbolic representations to explain their answers. There is no mention of such an overall trend of variation of representational use in extant literature, largely because there have been no studies that compare representational fluency across closely spaced levels of physics, or science, learning.

[1]  Kenneth R. Koedinger,et al.  Enhancing Middle School Students ’ Representational Fluency : A Classroom-Based Study , 2010 .

[2]  David F. Treagust,et al.  HIGH SCHOOL STUDENTS’ PROFICIENCY AND CONFIDENCE LEVELS IN DISPLAYING THEIR UNDERSTANDING OF BASIC ELECTROLYSIS CONCEPTS , 2012 .

[3]  David Hammer,et al.  Children's analogical reasoning in a third‐grade science discussion , 2006 .

[4]  Gaea Leinhardt,et al.  Functions, Graphs, and Graphing: Tasks, Learning, and Teaching , 1990 .

[5]  J. Elen,et al.  Conceptualising, investigating and stimulating representational flexibility in mathematical problem solving and learning: a critical review , 2009 .

[6]  John R. Thompson,et al.  Student Understanding Of The Physics And Mathematics Of Process Variables In P-V Diagrams , 2007 .

[7]  Manjula D. Sharma,et al.  Consistency of students' conceptions of wave propagation: Findings from a conceptual survey in mechanical waves , 2011 .

[8]  David E. Meltzer,et al.  Relation between students’ problem-solving performance and representational format , 2005 .

[9]  Mitchell J. Nathan,et al.  Representational disfluency in algebra: evidence from student gestures and speech , 2009 .

[10]  三嶋 博之 The theory of affordances , 2008 .

[11]  Brian Hand,et al.  Examining the Impact of Student Use of Multiple Modal Representations in Constructing Arguments in Organic Chemistry Laboratory Classes , 2010 .

[12]  Larry E. Toothaker,et al.  Multiple Comparison Procedures , 1992 .

[13]  Jim Woolnough,et al.  How do students learn to apply their mathematical knowledge to interpret graphs in physics? , 2000 .

[14]  Susan R. Goldman,et al.  Learning in complex domains: When and why do multiple representations help? , 2003 .

[15]  Yuri Uesaka,et al.  Active Comparison as a Means of Promoting the Development of Abstract Conditional Knowledge and Appropriate Choice of Diagrams in Math Word Problem Solving , 2006, Diagrams.

[16]  F. Aldrich,et al.  Graphicacy - the fourth 'R'? , 2000 .

[17]  N. Finkelstein,et al.  Patterns of multiple representation use by experts and novices during physics problem solving , 2008 .

[18]  Mike Stieff,et al.  Identifying Representational Competence With Multi-Representational Displays , 2011 .

[19]  John K. Gilbert,et al.  Visualization: A Metacognitive Skill in Science and Science Education , 2005 .

[20]  Maureen Lucy McDonough,et al.  A Case Study of the Transfer Process of a Selected Group of Students from a Community College to a Four-Year Teacher Education Program. , 2000 .

[21]  Robert J. Dufresne,et al.  Solving physics problems with multiple representations , 1997 .

[22]  S. Ainsworth DeFT: A Conceptual Framework for Considering Learning with Multiple Representations. , 2006 .

[23]  GEORGE M. BODNER,et al.  The Purdue Visualization of Rotations Test , 1997 .

[24]  C. Linder,et al.  Exploring the role of physics representations: an illustrative example from students sharing knowledge about refraction , 2012 .

[25]  Manjula D. Sharma,et al.  Use of interactive lecture demonstrations: A ten year study , 2010 .

[26]  John K. Gilbert,et al.  Visualization: An Emergent Field of Practice and Enquiry in Science Education , 2008 .

[27]  John Airey,et al.  A disciplinary discourse perspective on university science learning: Achieving fluency in a critical constellation of modes , 2009 .

[28]  Eugenia Etkina,et al.  Case Study: Students’ Use of Multiple Representations in Problem Solving , 2006 .

[29]  Wolff-Michael Roth,et al.  Differences in graph-related practices between high school biology textbooks and scientific ecology journals , 1999 .

[30]  Russell Tytler,et al.  Constructing representations to learn in science , 2013 .

[31]  A. diSessa Metarepresentation: Native Competence and Targets for Instruction , 2004 .

[32]  P. New,et al.  A case study of the transfer of mathematics skills by university students , 2005 .

[33]  John Airey,et al.  Developing and Evaluating a Survey for Representational Fluency in Science , 2014 .

[34]  J. Osborne,et al.  Supporting and Promoting Argumentation Discourse in Science Education , 2002 .

[35]  Robert J. Beichner,et al.  Testing student interpretation of kinematics graphs , 1994 .

[36]  P. Scott,et al.  Constructing Scientific Knowledge in the Classroom , 1994 .

[37]  Caroline Yoon,et al.  Evidence from cognitive neuroscience for the role of graphical and algebraic representations in understanding function , 2010 .

[38]  Wolff-Michael Roth,et al.  Of Cannibals, Missionaries, and Converts: Graphing Competencies from Grade 8 to Professional Science Inside (Classrooms) and Outside (Field/Laboratory) , 1999 .

[39]  Noah D. Finkelstein,et al.  Student Representational Competence and the Role of Instructional Environment in Introductory Physics , 2006 .

[40]  Glenda Jacobs Word usage misconceptions among first‐year university physics students , 1989 .

[41]  R. McCormick Conceptual and Procedural Knowledge , 1997 .

[42]  Noah D. Finkelstein,et al.  Effect of Instructional Environment on Physics Students' Representational Skills. , 2006 .

[43]  A. Field Discovering statistics using SPSS for Windows. , 2000 .

[44]  Wolff-Michael Roth,et al.  When Are Graphs Worth Ten Thousand Words? An Expert-Expert Study , 2003 .

[45]  Noah D. Finkelstein,et al.  Student representational competence and self-assessment when solving physics problems , 2005 .

[46]  Joseph Krajcik,et al.  Inscriptional Practices in Two Inquiry-Based Classrooms: A Case Study of Seventh Graders' Use of Data Tables and Graphs. , 2006 .

[47]  John K. Gilbert,et al.  Models and Modelling: Routes to More Authentic Science Education , 2004 .

[48]  A. Harding,et al.  Undergraduate students’ performance and confidence in procedural and conceptual mathematics , 2005 .

[49]  Wolff-Michael Roth,et al.  Interpretations of graphs by university biology students and practicing scientists: Toward a social practice view of scientific representation practices , 1999 .