Cognitive Control of Distinct Vocalizations in Rhesus Monkeys

Whether nonhuman primates can decouple their innate vocalizations from accompanied levels of arousal or specific events in the environment to achieve cognitive control over their vocal utterances has been a matter of debate for decades. We show that rhesus monkeys can be trained to elicit different call types on command in response to arbitrary visual cues. Furthermore, we report that a monkey learned to switch between two distinct call types from trial to trial in response to different visual cues. A controlled behavioral protocol and data analysis based on signal detection theory showed that noncognitive factors as a cause for the monkeys' vocalizations could be excluded. Our findings also suggest that monkeys also have rudimentary control over acoustic call parameters. These findings indicate that monkeys are able to volitionally initiate their vocal production and, therefore, are able to instrumentalize their vocal behavior to perform a behavioral task successfully.

[1]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Campbell's monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[2]  Monica Maranesi,et al.  Neurons Controlling Voluntary Vocalization in the Macaque Ventral Premotor Cortex , 2011, PloS one.

[3]  Michael Balter,et al.  Evolution of language. Animal communication helps reveal roots of language. , 2010, Science.

[4]  U. Jürgens,et al.  The neural control of vocalization in mammals: a review. , 2009, Journal of voice : official journal of the Voice Foundation.

[5]  Marc D. Hauser,et al.  Tracking silence: adjusting vocal production to avoid acoustic interference , 2007, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[6]  Akihiro Katada,et al.  Lombard reflex during PAG-induced vocalization in decerebrate cats , 1997, Neuroscience Research.

[7]  Kurt Hammerschmidt,et al.  Constraints in primate vocal production , 2008 .

[8]  Steffen R. Hage Audio–vocal interactions during vocal communication in squirrel monkeys and their neurobiological implications , 2013, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[9]  Sabyasachi Roy,et al.  Vocal control by the common marmoset in the presence of interfering noise , 2011, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[10]  U. Jürgens Neural pathways underlying vocal control , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[11]  Michael Heistermann,et al.  Competition-induced stress does not explain deceptive alarm calling in tufted capuchin monkeys , 2014, Animal Behaviour.

[12]  Uwe Jürgens,et al.  On the Role of the Pontine Brainstem in Vocal Pattern Generation: A Telemetric Single-Unit Recording Study in the Squirrel Monkey , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Basic mathematical rules are encoded by primate prefrontal cortex neurons , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[14]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour , 2015, PeerJ.

[15]  Brian Hare,et al.  Baboon Metaphysics: The Evolution of a Social Mind, Dorothy L. Cheney, Robert M. Seyfarth. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (2007), Pp. x+348. Price $27.50 paperback , 2008 .

[16]  Robert M. Seyfarth,et al.  Conceptual Semantics in a Nonhuman Primate , 1999 .

[17]  C. R. Larson,et al.  Cerebellar regulation of phonation in rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) , 1978, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  H. Brumm,et al.  The Lombard effect , 2011, Current Biology.

[19]  D. Rumbaugh,et al.  Symbolic communication between two chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 1978, Science.

[20]  Alex Martin,et al.  Species-specific calls activate homologs of Broca's and Wernicke's areas in the macaque , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  Signalers and receivers in animal communication. , 2003, Annual review of psychology.

[22]  D. Sutton,et al.  Primate phonation: Anterior cingulate lesion effects on response rate and acoustical structure , 1981, American journal of primatology.

[23]  A. Iriki,et al.  Spontaneous vocal differentiation of coo-calls for tools and food in Japanese monkeys , 2003, Neuroscience Research.

[24]  Asif A Ghazanfar,et al.  Language evolution: neural differences that make a difference , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[25]  Uwe Jürgens,et al.  Vocalization as an Emotional Indicator , 1979 .

[26]  U. Jürgens,et al.  Vocalization as an emotional indicator. A neuroethological study in the squirrel monkey. , 1979, Behaviour.

[27]  Hisae Gemba,et al.  Cortical field potentials associated with audio-initiated vocalization in monkeys , 1999, Neuroscience Letters.

[28]  K. Hammerschmidt,et al.  Responses of squirrel monkeys to their experimentally modified mobbing calls. , 2003, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[29]  Peter G. Aitken,et al.  Discriminative vocal conditioning in rhesus monkeys: Evidence for volitional control? , 1979, Brain and Language.

[30]  Jiang Feng,et al.  Ambient noise induces independent shifts in call frequency and amplitude within the Lombard effect in echolocating bats , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[31]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[32]  H. de Vries,et al.  Male monkeys remember which group members have given alarm calls , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  D B Moody,et al.  Regulation of voice amplitude by the monkey. , 1975, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[34]  N. Logothetis,et al.  A voice region in the monkey brain , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  C. Larson,et al.  Vocalization in rhesus monkeys: conditionability. , 1973, Brain research.

[36]  H. Brumm,et al.  Acoustic Communication in Noise , 2005 .

[37]  P. Slater,et al.  The different roles of social learning in vocal communication , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[38]  P. Marler,et al.  Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): I. Socioecological factors , 1993 .

[39]  D. Rendall,et al.  The meaning and function of grunt variants in baboons , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[40]  John D. Pierce,et al.  A review of attempts to condition operantly alloprimate vocalizations , 1985, Primates.

[41]  A. Nieder Prefrontal cortex and the evolution of symbolic reference , 2009, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[42]  R. Seyfarth,et al.  Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations , 2010, Brain and Language.

[43]  Hiroki Koda,et al.  Experimental evidence for the volitional control of vocal production in an immature gibbon , 2007 .

[44]  B. Skinner,et al.  Symbolic Communication Between Two Pigeons, (Columba livia domestica) , 1980, Science.

[45]  Steffen R. Hage Chapter 8.3 - Neuronal networks involved in the generation of vocalization , 2010 .

[46]  Steffen R. Hage,et al.  Audio–vocal interaction in the pontine brainstem during self‐initiated vocalization in the squirrel monkey , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[47]  P. Marler,et al.  Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. , 1980, Science.

[48]  Cory T. Miller,et al.  Antiphonal call timing in marmosets is behaviorally significant: interactive playback experiments , 2009, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[49]  D. Todt,et al.  Acoustic communication in noise: regulation of call characteristics in a New World monkey , 2004, Journal of Experimental Biology.

[50]  H. Brumm,et al.  The evolution of the Lombard effect: 100 years of psychoacoustic research , 2011 .

[51]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  Semantic Associations between Signs and Numerical Categories in the Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, PLoS biology.

[52]  Robert M. Seyfarth,et al.  Suricate alarm calls signal predator class and urgency , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[53]  M. Petrides,et al.  Orofacial somatomotor responses in the macaque monkey homologue of Broca's area , 2005, Nature.