Oculomotor tracking in two dimensions.

Results from studies of oculomotor tracking in one dimension have indicated that saccades are driven primarily by errors in position, whereas smooth pursuit movements are driven primarily by errors in velocity. To test whether this result generalizes to two-dimensional tracking, we asked subjects to track a target that moved initially in a straight line then changed direction. We found that the general premise does indeed hold true; however, the study of oculomotor tracking in two dimensions provides additional insight. The first saccade was directed slightly in advance of target location at saccade onset. Thus its direction was related primarily to angular positional error. The direction of the smooth pursuit movement after the saccade was related linearly to the direction of target motion with an average slope of 0.8. Furthermore the magnitude and direction of smooth pursuit velocity did not change abruptly; consequently the direction of smooth pursuit appeared to rotate smoothly over time.

[1]  D. Robinson The mechanics of human smooth pursuit eye movement. , 1965, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  E. L. Keller,et al.  Generation of smooth-pursuit eye movements: neuronal mechanisms and pathways , 1991, Neuroscience Research.

[3]  R. Kettner,et al.  Predictive smooth pursuit of complex two-dimensional trajectories demonstrated by perturbation responses in monkeys , 1997, Vision Research.

[4]  J. Sharpe,et al.  Smooth pursuit initiation in young and elderly subjects , 1993, Vision Research.

[5]  R. Gellman,et al.  Human smooth pursuit: stimulus-dependent responses. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. , 1969, Vision research.

[7]  M. Flanders,et al.  Arm movements in three-dimensional space: computation, theory, and observation. , 1991, Exercise and sport sciences reviews.

[8]  C. Rashbass,et al.  The relationship between saccadic and smooth tracking eye movements , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  A. Georgopoulos On reaching. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[10]  T. Ebner,et al.  Temporal encoding of movement kinematics in the discharge of primate primary motor and premotor neurons. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  H. Noda,et al.  Eye position signals in the flocculus of the monkey during smooth‐pursuit eye movements , 1982, The Journal of physiology.

[12]  Jacques Droulez,et al.  Target velocity based prediction in saccadic vector programming , 1989, Vision Research.

[13]  P Viviani,et al.  The Relationship between Curvature and Velocity in Two-Dimensional Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  S. Lisberger,et al.  Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  D. Robinson,et al.  A METHOD OF MEASURING EYE MOVEMENT USING A SCLERAL SEARCH COIL IN A MAGNETIC FIELD. , 1963, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[16]  J. T. Massey,et al.  Mental rotation of the neuronal population vector. , 1989, Science.

[17]  Harry J. Wyatt,et al.  Target position and velocity: The stimuli for smooth pursuit eye movements , 1980, Vision Research.

[18]  E. J. Morris,et al.  Visual motion processing and sensory-motor integration for smooth pursuit eye movements. , 1987, Annual review of neuroscience.

[19]  S G Lisberger,et al.  Vector Averaging for Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements Initiated by Two Moving Targets in Monkeys , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.