Rehabilitation of executive dysfunction following brain injury: “Content-free” cueing improves everyday prospective memory performance

Prospective memory (PM) is often claimed to rely upon executive as well as mnemonic resources. Here, we examined the contribution of executive functions towards PM by providing intermittent support for monitoring processes using "content-free" cues, which carried no direct information regarding the PM task itself. Twenty participants with non-progressive brain injury and PM difficulties received brief training in linking a cue phrase "STOP!" with pausing current activity and reviewing stored goals. The efficacy of this strategy was examined with a PM task requiring participants to make telephone calls to a voicemail service at four set times each day for 10 days. Task content was encoded using errorless learning to minimise retrospective memory-based failures. On five randomly selected days, eight text messages reading simply "STOP!" were sent to participants' mobile telephones, but crucially not within an hour of a target time. Striking improvements in performance were observed on cued days, thus demonstrating a within-subjects experimental modulation of PM performance using cues that carry no information other than by association with participants' stored memory of their intentions. In addition to the theoretical insights, the time course over which the effect was observed constitutes encouraging evidence that such strategies are useful in helping to remediate some negative consequences of executive dysfunction. It is proposed that this benefit results from enhanced efficiency of goal management via increased monitoring of current and future goals, and the steps necessary to achieve them, perhaps compensating for under-functioning fronto-parietal attention systems.

[1]  C. Frith,et al.  Brain regions involved in prospective memory as determined by positron emission tomography , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  J. Singer,et al.  Incentive Effects in Prospective Remembering , 1977 .

[3]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Aging and prospective memory: examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes. , 1995 .

[4]  R. Kawashima,et al.  Participation of the prefrontal cortices in prospective memory: evidence from a PET study in humans , 1998, Neuroscience Letters.

[5]  R. Reitan Validity of the Trail Making Test as an Indicator of Organic Brain Damage , 1958 .

[6]  O. Spreen,et al.  A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary , 1991 .

[7]  I. Robertson,et al.  The absent mind: further investigations of sustained attention to response , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  F. Craik,et al.  Virtual week and actual week: Age‐related differences in prospective memory , 2000 .

[9]  J. Evans,et al.  External cueing systems in the rehabilitation of executive impairments of action , 1998, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[10]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory , 1995, Nature.

[11]  I. Robertson,et al.  Rehabilitation of executive function: facilitation of effective goal management on complex tasks using periodic auditory alerts , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  B. Thompson,et al.  MR imaging of head trauma: review of the distribution and radiopathologic features of traumatic lesions. , 1988, AJR. American journal of roentgenology.

[13]  Jonathan J. Evans,et al.  Who makes good use of memory aids? Results of a survey of people with acquired brain injury , 2003, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[14]  A. Thöne-Otto,et al.  Disentangling executive functions and memory processes in event-based prospective remembering after brain damage: A neuropsychological study , 2003 .

[15]  H. Levin,et al.  Long-term neuropsychological outcome and loss of social autonomy after traumatic brain injury. , 1997, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[16]  L. Rapport,et al.  Measures of executive functioning as predictors of functional ability and social integration in a rehabilitation sample. , 1999, Archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation.

[17]  Rebekah E. Smith The cost of remembering to remember in event-based prospective memory: investigating the capacity demands of delayed intention performance. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  S. Scott,et al.  The role of the rostral frontal cortex (area 10) in prospective memory: a lateral versus medial dissociation , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  B. Milner Some cognitive effects of frontal-lobe lesions in man. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[20]  Barbara A. Wilson,et al.  Clinical Management of Memory Problems , 1992 .

[21]  T. Shallice,et al.  Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[22]  J Duncan,et al.  Rehabilitation of executive functioning: An experimental–clinical validation of Goal Management Training , 2000, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[23]  C. Camp,et al.  Prospective memory intervention in Alzheimer's disease. , 1992, Journal of gerontology.

[24]  M. Lezak Neuropsychological assessment, 3rd ed. , 1995 .

[25]  Michael Cannizzaro,et al.  Traumatic Brain Injury: A Primer for Professionals , 2002 .

[26]  Peter La Cour,et al.  Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test , 1990 .

[27]  D. V. Cramon,et al.  Back to work with a chronic dysexecutive syndrome? (A case report) , 1994 .

[28]  B. Wilson Memory therapy in practice , 1992 .

[29]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[30]  J. Duncan,et al.  Intelligence and the Frontal Lobe: The Organization of Goal-Directed Behavior , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  Jonathan J. Evans,et al.  Prospective memory functioning in people with and without brain injury , 2002, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[32]  I. Robertson,et al.  An fMRI of sustained attention with endogenous and exogenous engagement. , 2004, Brain and cognition.

[33]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Multiple processes in prospective memory retrieval: factors determining monitoring versus spontaneous retrieval. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[34]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  A power primer. , 1992, Psychological bulletin.

[35]  M. Kerszberg,et al.  A Neuronal Model of a Global Workspace in Effortful Cognitive Tasks , 2001 .

[36]  Norbert Mai,et al.  Problem-solving deficits in brain-injured patients: A therapeutic approach , 1991 .

[37]  Tom Manly,et al.  An electronic knot in the handkerchief: “Content free cueing” and the maintenance of attentive control , 2004 .

[38]  J. Ellis Prospective memory or the realization of delayed intentions: A conceptual framework for research. , 1996 .

[39]  Barbara A Wilson,et al.  Preliminary report of a NeuroPage service within a health care system. , 2003, NeuroRehabilitation.

[40]  M. McDaniel,et al.  The involvement of executive functions in prospective memory performance of adults , 2003 .

[41]  H. Emslie,et al.  Evaluation of NeuroPage: a new memory aid , 1997, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[42]  T Shallice,et al.  The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behaviour. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[43]  J. Duncan EPS Mid-Career Award 2004: Brain mechanisms of attention , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[44]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[45]  I. Robertson,et al.  `Oops!': Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[46]  Alan D. Baddeley,et al.  Errorless learning in the rehabilitation of memory impaired people , 1994 .

[47]  Michaela Swales,et al.  Frontal Lobe Deficits after Head Injury: Unity and Diversity of Function , 1997 .

[48]  J. Evans,et al.  Reducing everyday memory and planning problems by means of a paging system: a randomised control crossover study , 2001, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[49]  P. Burgess,et al.  The case for the development and use of “ecologically valid” measures of executive function in experimental and clinical neuropsychology , 2006, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[50]  T. Perneger What's wrong with Bonferroni adjustments , 1998, BMJ.

[51]  Donald A. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action , 1986 .

[52]  J. Duncan,et al.  Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.

[53]  Barbara A. Wilson,et al.  Errorless learning of face-name associations in early Alzheimer's disease , 1999 .

[54]  B. Wilson Ecological validity of neuropsychological assessment: Do neuropsychological indexes predict performance in everyday activities? , 1993 .